对立违抗性障碍、注意缺陷多动障碍儿童冲动行为的心理社会因素和TPH2、5-HTTLPR及MAOA基因多态性研究
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
第一部分Barratt冲动量表的信效度研究
     目的
     采用Barratt冲动量表(Barratt Impulsiveness Scale,BIS)界定儿童冲动行为,对Barratt冲动量表进行信效度检验。
     方法
     BIS-11由美国学者Barratt编制,由作者翻译为中文版。采用系统抽样方法选取1200名7~12岁的学生完成BIS-11中文版的测试,使用修定后的BIS-11中文版量表进行探索性因素分析与验证性因素分析,并且用Cronbach a系数、分半信度系数和重测信度系数进行信度检验。
     结果
     1.BIS-11包括3个因素:注意力冲动性、运动冲动性和无计划冲动性。采用Lickter 4点量表记分,由1分(从不/很少)到4分(几乎总是/总是),总分在24-96分之间。
     2.BIS-11的Cronbach a系数为0.86,分半信度系数为0.82,重测信度系数为0.91。量表包括的3个因素共解释方差41.72%,验证性因素分析显示量表的结构效度较好,RMSEA=0.049。
     结论BIS-11中文版具有较好的信效度,是一种较好的儿童冲动行为测评工具。
     第二部分冲动行为儿童的心理社会因素对照研究
     目的
     探讨儿童冲动行为与心理社会因素的关系,为儿童冲动行为的干预提供依据。
     方法
     采用病例对照研究,对157例具有冲动行为的儿童和152例正常对照组的儿童,采用一般人口学指标、父母养育方式评价量表、Barratt冲动量表中文版、儿童气质问卷、Piers-Harris儿童自我意识量表、自尊调查量表、Conners教师问卷、家庭功能评定问卷进行评定。并对儿童冲动行为多种影响因素进行t检验、卡方检验和Binary Logestic回归分析。
     结果
     1、研究组儿童学习成绩欠佳者显著多于对照组(P=0.04);研究组家庭关系中父母关系不良者显著多于对照组(P=0.00),且发生争执者较多(P=0.02)。
     2、研究组父母对孩子的情感温暖及理解分值显著低于对照组(依次P=0.01,P=0.02),父母对孩子的严厉及惩罚、否认及拒绝的分值显著高于对照组,两组差异有显著性(依次P=0.02,P=0.04,P=0.00,P=0.00)。
     3.研究组运动冲动性、无计划冲动性及冲动总分均比对照组严重,差异有显著性意义(依次P=0.00,P=0.00,P=0.00);
     4、在气质特点方面,研究组在活动水平、预测性、反应强度、心境特征、持久性、注意分散维度分值较高,与对照组比较差异有显著性(依次P=0.00,P=0.00,P=0.00,P=0.03,P=0.01,P=0.00),研究组反应阈显著比对照组低(P=0.00);而趋避性及适应性差异无显著性(P=0.35,P=0.06)。研究组的气质类型以麻烦型(Difficulty,D型)及中间近麻烦型(Intermediate-Difficuhy,I-D型)较多(P=0.00,P=0.00)。
     5、研究组在行为、焦虑、合群、幸福与满足、总分方面的评分较低,与对照组相比差异均有显著性(P=0.00);而在智力与学校情况、躯体外貌与属性与对照组比较差异无显著性(依次P=0.15,P=0.44)。
     6、研究组自尊总分较低,与对照组比较差异有显著性(P=0.01)。
     7、研究组Conners教师问卷(Teacher Rating Scale,TRS)的3个因子分及多动指数均比对照组高,研究组在品行问题、多动、注意不集中-被动及多动指数与对照组相比差异均有显著性(依次P=0.02,P=0.00,P=0.03,P=0.00)
     8、研究组家庭功能(FAD)中,情感介入、行为控制和总的功能分值显著高于对照组(P=0.03,P=0.01,P=0.03)
     9、儿童冲动行为Binary Logestic回归分析,最终依次进入回归方程的是夫妻关系,父亲对孩子的情感温暖及理解,母亲的否认及拒绝,运动冲动性,活动水平,适应性,反应强度,反应阈,焦虑,注意不集中-被动,多动指数,问题解决和情感介入。其中夫妻关系、父亲对孩子的情感温暖及理解,母亲的否认及拒绝,运动冲动性、活动水平、适应性、反应强度、反应阈、注意不集中-被动、多动指数、问题解决、情感介入具有统计学意义(P<0.05)。
     结论
     1、冲动行为的儿童学习成绩欠佳的较多;家庭关系中父母关系不良的较多,且易发生争执。
     2、冲动行为儿童具有一定的气质基础,父母不良的养育方式较多,自我意识及自尊普遍较低。
     3、冲动行为儿童家庭功能紊乱的较多,具有过度的情感介入及行为控制。
     4、儿童冲动行为与父母关系、父亲对孩子缺乏情感温暖及理解的养育方式、对新环境的适应能力、对新刺激的反应阈值、注意不集中、母亲的否认及拒绝、运动冲动性、活动水平、反应强度、多动指数、问题解决、情感介入有关。其中父母关系,父亲对孩子缺乏情感温暖及理解的养育方式、对新环境的适应能力、对新刺激的反应阈值及注意不集中均是危险因素,可增加儿童期发生冲动行为的危险。
     第三部分冲动行为儿童的分子生物学研究
     目的
     探讨儿童冲动行为可能的易感基因。
     方法
     采用病例对照研究的方法,对148例具有冲动行为的儿童和152例正常对照组,采用基因芯片杂交技术检测与TPH2基因功能密切关联的4个SNPs多态性位点:rs4570625、rs11178997、rs1386494和rs7305115,用RFLP技术检测两组5-HTTLPR和MAOA-uVNTR多态性,对所得基因型和等位基因频率进行关联分析,以探讨其与冲动行为的关联。
     结果
     ①研究组rs4570625的基因型分布与对照组不同,基因型为纯合子T/T的显著较多,与对照组比较差异有显著性(x~2=4.051,P=0.044),其OR值为1.708,即研究组儿童rs4570625基因型为纯合子T/T而产生冲动行为的危险度是对照组的1.708倍,其总体相对危险度的95%可信区间为1.012-2.884。rs11178997的基因型A/A、A/T、T/T两组间差异无显著性(依次P=0.642,P=0.257,P=0.218)。rs4570625及rs11178997的等位基因频率与对照组比较差异均无显著性(依次P=0.174,P=0.222);②经rs4570625及rs11178997两个基因型联合风险比较显示:研究组rs4570625及rs11178997 SNPs位点TT-AT两个基因型联合风险与对照组不同,研究组的TT-AT联合显著比对照组多,两组比较差异有统计学意义(x~2=4.197,P=0.040),rs4570625及rs11178997基因型为TT-AT联合形式的OR值为2.350,即研究组rs4570625及rs11178997基因型为TT-AT联合而产生冲动行为的危险度是对照组的2.350倍,其总体相对危险度的95%可信区间为1.018-5.424。其它SNPs位点两个基因型的八种联合形式的风险两组比较差异均无显著性(P均>0.05);③研究组rs1386494的基因型分别为AA、AG及GG,其三种基因型分布与对照组比较,差异无显著性(依次P=0.308,P=0.338,P=0.442)。研究组rs7305115的基因型亦分别为AA、AG及GG,其三种基因型分布与对照组比较,差异无显著性(P=0.291,P=0.843,P=0.201)。两组rs1386494及rs7305115等位基因频率及基因型联合风险均无统计学意义;④研究组5-HTTLPR多态性的基因型分别为LL、SS、SL、SL~+及LL~+,在研究组还发现了两例SL~(++)的特殊基因型,两组基因型比较均无统计学意义。研究组5-HTTLPR等位基因L频率显著较少,与对照组比较差异有显著性(x~2=4.092,P=0.043);⑤发现了MAOA-uVNTR的4.5R、5.5R两种新的基因型。研究组4.5/4.5基因型显著较对照组少(P=0.004);等位基因为5R的频率显著较对照组多,与对照组比较差异有显著性(P=0.004),其OR值为1.979,即研究组儿童MAOA-uVNTR多态性的等位基因为5R而产生冲动行为的危险度是对照组的1.979倍,其总体相对危险度的95%可信区间为1.244-3.150;⑥研究组男童MAOA-uVNTR基因型为5/5较多,与对照组比较差异有显著性(P=0.006),其OR值为3.997,即研究组儿童MAOA-uVNTR多态性的等位基因为5R而产生冲动行为的危险度是对照组的3.997倍,其总体相对危险度的95%可信区间为1.400-11.405。研究组男童MAOA-uVNTR等位基因5R的频率较多,与对照组比较差异有显著性(P=0.000)。其OR值为3.991,即研究组男性儿童MAOA-uVNTR多态性的等位基因为5R而产生冲动行为的危险度是对照组的3.991倍,其总体相对危险度的95%可信区间为1.956-8.144。
     结论
     ①rs4570625基因型为纯合子T/T时增加了儿童冲动行为的风险,其危险度是对照组的1.708倍。rs4570625与rs11178997基因型为TT-AT的联合可增加了儿童冲动行为的风险,其危险度是对照组的2.350倍;②冲动行为儿童5-HTTLPR等位基因L频率显著较少;③发现了MAOA-uVNTR的4.5R、5.5R两种新的基因型。冲动行为儿童4.5/4.5基因型显著较少、等位基因为5R的频率显著较多,分析认为当等位基因为5R时可增加儿童冲动行为的风险,其危险度是对照组的1.979倍;④研究组男童MAOA-uVNTR基因型为5/5较多,其发生冲动行为的危险度是对照组的3.997倍。⑤以上结果提示TPH2基因SNP多态性、5-HTTLPR和MAOA-uVNTR基因多态性与儿童冲动行为有直接和/或间接联系。
PartⅠResearch of Reliability and Validity for BarrattImpulsiveness Scale (BIS-11)
     Objectives
     To explore the reliability and validity of Barratt Impulsiveness Scalein order to select the children with impulsive behaviour.
     Methods
     Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) was compiled by Americanscholar Barratt, it was translated into Chinese by author. 1200 studentswhich age range from 7 to 12 years old were selected by systematicsampling. All of them tested by revised Chinese BIS-11. The explosivefactor analysis and the confirmative factor analysis were made on revisedChinese BIS-11. And the reliability testing was made with Cronbach acoefficient, split-half reliability coefficient and test-retest reliabilitycoefficient.
     Results
     1. The BIS-11 include three factors, they are attentionalimpulsiveness (Iat), motor impulsiveness (Im) and non-planningimpulsiveness (Inp). We used the four-point variable method of Lickter todesign the scale, from 1 (never/rarely) to 4 (almost/always), the total score is from 24 to 96.
     2. The Cronbach a coefficient of BIS-11's is 0.86, the split-halfreliability coefficient is 0.82, and the test-retest reliability coefficient is0.91. The three factors of scale explained 41.72%of the variance. Theresults of confirmative factor analysis showed satisfactory constructionvalidity (RMSEA=0.067).
     Conclusions
     The BIS-11 Chinese Revision is a better children impulsivenessscale which achieves satisfactory reliability and validity.
     PartⅡContrast Research of Social PsychologicalFactors For Impulsive Children
     Objectives
     To explore the relationship between children impulsiveness andsocial psychological factors, in order to provide a basis for interventionof impulsiveness.
     Methods
     157 impulsive children and 152 normal children were researched bycase-control study with the demography index, the Egm Minnen avBardnosnauppforstran (EMBU), the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11Chinese Revision (BIS-11CR), Behavioral Style questionnaire (BSQ),Piers-Harris Children's Self-concept Scale (PHCSS), The Self-esteemInventory (SEI), Conners Teacher Rating Scale (TRS) and FmamilyAsessment Device (FAD). The multiple influence factors were analysisedby t-test, x~2 test and Binary Logestic regression analysis.
     Results
     1. The number of poor-school record children in study group werehigher than control group (P=0.04), and the number of children whichparents' relationship was not good were significantly higher than incontrol group (P=0.00), especially argument (P=0.02).
     2. The scores of feeling warmness and understanding in study groupwere significantly lower than control group (P=0.01, P=0.02). The severity, over-punishment, denial and refusal scores of study group weresignificantly higher than control group (P=0.02, P=0.04, P=0.00, P=0.00).
     3. The scores of attentional impulsiveness and non-planningimpulsiveness and the total scores in study group were significantlyhigher than control group (P=0.00, P=0.00, P=0.00).
     4. On the temperament characteristic, the activity level,predictability and rhythmicity, intensity of reaction, feature of mood,persistency, attention-dispersal scores of study group were significantlyhigher than control group (P=0.00, P=0.00, P=0.00, P=0.03, P=0.01, P=0.00). The score of threshold of reaction in study group wassignificantly lower than control group (P=0.00). There were nosignificant differences in approach-withdrawal and adaptability scoresbetween study group and control group (P=0.35, P=0.06). Mostchildren's temperament in study group were Difficulty (D) andIntermediate-Difficulty (I-D) type, and there were significant differencesbetween study group and control group (P=0.00, P=0.00).
     5. The scores of behavior, anxiety, gregariousness, happiness andsatisfaction and total scores in study group were significantly lower thancontrol group (P=0.00). There were no significant differences in thescores of intelligence and school situation, body and appearance betweenstudy group and control group (P=0.15, P=0.44).
     6. The total scores of self-esteem in study group were significantlylower than control group (P=0.01).
     7. The scores of Teacher Rating Scale' s three factors which includeconduct, hyperactivity, impaired concentration and passive as well ashyperactivity index in study group were significantly higher than controlgroup(P=0.02, P=0.00, P=0.03, P=0.00).
     8. On the FAD, the scores of affective involvement, behaviorcontrol and general functioning in study group were significantly higherthan control group (P=0.03, P=0.01, P=0.03).
     9. The children impulsiveness were analyzed by Binary Logesticregression analysis, whom entered the regression equation arerelationship of husband and wife, father's feeling warmness andunderstanding, mother's denial and refusal, motor impulsiveness, activitylevel, adaptability, intensity of reaction, threshold of reaction, anxiety,impaired concentration and passive, hyperactivity index, problem solvingand affective involvement. In addition to anxiety, the other factors hadstatistical significance (P<0.05).
     Conclusions
     1. Most impulsiveness children had poor-school record, and theirparents' relationship were not good, especially argument.
     2. Impulsiveness children had temperament basis. They had lowerself-awareness and self-esteem than normal and their parents had harmful parental rearing patterns.
     3. The family of impulsiveness children were more dysfunction thannormal, and they had excessivly affective involvement and behaviorcontrol.
     4. Impulsiveness was related to some factors which include children'parents relationship, father's lack of feeling warmness and understanding,adaptability for new surroundings, threshold of reaction for newstimulation, impaired concentration, mother's denial and refusal, motorimpulsiveness, activity level, intensity of reaction, hyperactivity index,problem solving, affective involvement. Among which, parentsrelationship, father's lack of feeling warmness and understanding,adaptability for new surroundings, threshold of reaction for newstimulation and impaired concentration were dangerous factors, theycontributed to children impulsiveness.
     PartⅢMolecular-biollgical study on childrenimpulsive behaviour
     Objectives
     To detect the potential predisposing genes of children who sufferedfrom impulsive behaviour.
     Methods
     148 impulsiveness children and 152 healthy control subjects werestudied by the approach of case control. Gene chip hybridizationtechnique were performed to detect polymorphic sites of 4 singlenucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs): rs4570625、rs11178997、rs1386494and rs7305115, which was associated with TPH-2 gene. To detect thepolymorphism of two sets 5-HTTLPR and MAOA-uVNTR withrestriction fragment length polymorphism technology (RFLP), and thenthe genotype and allele frequency were analyzed by association analysisin order to detect the association of impulsive behaviour and predisposinggenes.
     Results
     ①There is difference in genotype distribution of rs4570625,that thegenotype is homozygote T/T is much higher in the investigated group.Compared with the control group, there is a signifcantdifference(x~2=4.051, P=0.044), the OR is 1.708.That is, in the investigatedgroup, the risk of suffering from impulsive behaviour is 1.708 times than the control group when the genotype of rs4570625 is homozygote T/T,the popular relative risk's 95%confidence interval is 1.012-2.884. In thetwo groups, there is no difference in genotype A/A,A/T,T/T ofrs11178997(by turns P=0.642, P=0.257, P=0.218).Also there is nodifference in the allele frequency of rs4570625 and rs11178997 in the twogroups(by turns P=0.174,P=0.222).
     ②Through risky comparison of the combination of the twogenotypes in rs4570625 and rs11178997, it shows: compared with thecontrol group, there is a difference in the locus TT-AT combination ofrs4570625 and rs11178997 in the investigated group, the TT-ATcombination of the investigated group is significantly higher than thecontrol group, there is statistic significance in the two groups'comparison(x~2=4.197, P=0.040), the OR is 2.350 in the genotype TT-ATcombinations of rs4570625 and rs11178997. That is, in the investigatedgroup, because of the genotype TT-AT combinations of rs4570625 andrs11178997, the risk suffering from impulsive behaviour is 2.350 timesthan the control group, the popular relative risk's 95%confidence intervalis 1.018-5.424.There is no difference in the eight combination forms ofthe two genotypes in other SNPs locus in the two groups(all P>0.05).
     ③In the investigated group, the genotype of rs1386494 isrespectively AA, AG and GG, the distribution of the three genotypes hasno signifecance compared with the control group(by turns P=0.308, P=0.338, P=0.442). Also the genotype of rs7305115 is AA, AG and GG,there is no significance compared with the control group(by turnsP=0.291, P=0.843, P=0.201). The allele frequency and genotypecombination risk of the two rs1386494 and rs7305115 in the two groupsalso have no statistical significance.
     ④In the investigated group, the genotype of 5-HTTLPRpolymorphism is respectively LL,SS,SL,SL~+ and LL~+, we also discoveredtwo samples whose genotype was special SL~(++), there is no statisticalsignificance in the comparison of genotype in the two groups. Thefrequency of 5-HTTLPR allelomorphic gene L is significantly lower inthe investigated group, compared with the control group, there is asignificant difference(x~2=4.092, P=0.043).
     ⑤We also discovered two new genotypes 4.5R and 5.5R inMAOA-uVNTR. In the investigated group, the genotype 4.5/4.5 issignificantly lower compared with the control group(P=0.004);Thefrequency of allele 5R is significantly higher in the investigated group,there is a statistical significance in the two groups(P=0.004),the OR is1.979. That is, in the investigated group, the risk is 1.979 times than thecontrol group in children whose allelomorphic gene of MAOA-uVNTRpolymorphism is 5R may suffer from impulsive behaviour, the popularrelative risk's 95%confidence interval is 1.244-3.150.
     ⑥In the boys of the investigated group, the genotype 5/5 of MAOA-uVNTR is much higher, there is a statistical significance in thetwo groups(P=0.006),the OR is 3.997.That is, the risk is 3.997 times thanthe control group in children whose allelomorphic gene ofMAOA-uVNTR polymorphism is 5R may suffer from impulsivebehaviour, the popular relative risk's 95%confidence interval is1.400-11.405. In the boys of investigated group, the frequency ofallelomorphic gene 5R of MAOA-uVNTR is much higher, there is astatistical significance in the two groups(P=0.000), the OR is 3.991That is, the risk is 3.991 times than the control group in children whoseallelomorphic gene of MAOA-uVNTR polymorphism is 5R may sufferfrom impulsive behaviour, the popular relative risk's 95%confidenceinterval is 1.956-8.144.
     Conclusions
     ①That the genotype of rs4570625 is homozygote T/T added therisk of impulsive behaviour in children, and the risk is 1.708 times thanthe control group; It may also add the risk when the genotype ofrs4570625 and rs11178997 is a combination of TT-AT.
     ②The frequency of 5-HTTLPR allelomorphic gene L issignificantly lower in children who suffered from impulsive behaviour.
     ③We discovered two new genotypes of MAOA-uVNTR:4.5R and5.5R.The 4.5/4.5 genotype is significantly lower in children who sufferedfrom impulsive behaviour, while the frequency of allele 5R is much higher. We consider it adds the risk of impulsive behaviour when thegenotype is 5R.,the risk 1.979 times over the control group.
     ④In the boys of the investigated group, it is much higher whengenotype of MAOA-uVNTR is 5/5,the risk is 3.997 times than the controlgroup.
     ⑤The upper results indicate there is a direct and/or indirectassociation about SNP polymorphism of TPH2,5HTTLPR, MAOA-uVNTR and children's impulsive behaviour.
引文
[1] Seroczynski AD. Bergeman CS. Coccaro EF.Etiology of the impulsivity raggression relationship:Genes or environment?Psychiatry Research, 1999,86: 41-57
    
    [2] Lesch KP, Merschdorf U.Impulsivity, Aggression, and Serotonin: Amolecular Psychobiological Perspective.Behavioral Sciences and the Law,2000,18: 581-604
    [3] EstibalizA,Carmen S.Impulsivity:areview.Psicothema2006,18(2):213-220
    [4] Daruna JH,Barnes PH.A neurodevelopment view of impulsive.In McCown WG,Johnson JL.Shure MB(EDS) The impulsive client theory ,research and treatment. American Psychological Association, Washington DC, 1999, 2-101
     [5] Moeller FG,Barratt ES,Dougherty DM,et al.Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity,Am J Psychiatry,2001,158:1783-1793
    [6] Kreek ML Nielsen DA, Butelman ER, LaForge KS. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci. 2005 ,8(11): 1450-1457.
    [7] Butcher JN, Rouse SV. Personality: individual differences and clinical assessment. Annual Review of Psychology, 1996;47:87- 111
    [8] Evenden JL.Varieties of impulsivity.Psychophacology,1999 146:348-361
    [9] King TZ,Fennell EB,Bauer R,et al. MMPI-2 profiles of patients with intractable epilepsy, Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2002 Aug;17(6):583-593
    [10] Sharpe JP,Desai S.The Revised Neo Personality Inventory and the MMPI-2 Psychopathology Five in the prediction of aggression. Personality and Individual Differences,2001,31:505-518
    
    [11] Alexander AV,The MMPI and chronic pain:a conceptual analysis of a long-standing but complicated relationship.Clinical Psychology Review,2000,20(5):533-599
    
    [12] Schmeck K, Poustka F. Temperament and disruptive behavior disorders. Psychopathology, 2001:34:159-163.
    [13] Kochanska G, Murray KT, Harlan ET.Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development.Dev Psychol. 2000 Mar;36(2):220-232.
    [14] Barkley RA.Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 1997 Jan;121(1):65-94.
    [15] 李雪荣主编.现代儿童精神医学 湖南科技出版社 1994.
    [16] Carlson CL,Tamm L,Gaub M.Gender differences in children with ADHD, ODD, and co-occuring ADHD/ODD identified in a school population[J].J Am Acad Child Adolesce Psychiatry, 1997,36(12):1706-1714.
    [17] Burke JD,Loeber R and Birmaher B.Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder:A review of the past 10 years,part Ⅱ. J Am Acad Child Adolesce Psychiatry.2002,41: (11): 1275-1293
    [18] Jensen PS,Martin D,Cantwell DP.Comorbidity in ADHD implications for research. Practice and DSM-Ⅳ[J].J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 1997,36:1065-1079.
    [19] 孙凌,苏林雁,刘永忠.长沙市中小学生对立违抗障碍的现况及对研究[J].中华精神科杂志,2001,34(4):208-211
    [20] 王玉凤,任桂英,顾伯美.不同时期北京市城区儿童行为问题比较研究.中国心理卫生杂志,2000.14(1):51-53
    [21] Rutter M. Isle of Wight revisited: twenty-five years of child psychiatric epidemiology.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 1989 Sep;28(5):633-653
    [22] Lemer JA, Inui TS, Trupin EW, Douglas E. Preschool behavior can predict future psychiatric disorders.J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1985 Jan;24(1):42-48.
    [23] Langner TS, Gersten JC, McCarthy ED, Eisenberg JG, Greene EL, Herson JH, Jameson JD. A screening inventory for assessing psychiatric impairment in children 6 to 18. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1976 Apr;44(2):286-296.
    [24] Bilenberg N, Horder K. Behavioral symptoms among children and adolescents. Screening with the help of a questionnaire in a group of children aged 4 to 17 years. Ugeskr Laeger. 1998 Jul 20; 160(30):4423-4428.
    [25] 忻仁娥,唐慧琴,张志雄等.全国22个省市26个单位24013名城市在校少年儿童行为问题调查(独生子女精神卫生问题的调查,防治和Achenbach儿童行为量表中国标准化).上海精神医学,1992,新4(1):47-65
    [26] 高雪屏,于素维,苏林雁.儿童青少年多动冲动行为与父母养育方式的关系,中国临床心理学杂志,2003;11(1):36-38
    [27] Hechtman L.Families of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A review. Can J Psychiatry, 1996,41(6):350-360
    [28] Lorenz K.On aggression.New York:Harcourt,brace, & World. 1996
    [29] Miles DR, Carey G. Genetic and environmental archiecture of human aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1997,72(1):207-217.
    [30] Randy J.Nelson SC. Molecular basis of aggression. TRENDS in Neurosciences , 2001,24(12):713-719
    [31] Nina K. Popova. From genes to aggressive behavior: the role of serotonergic system. BioEssays, 2006,28(5):495-503
    [32]Shih JC, Grimsby J, Chen K.Molecular biology of monoamine oxidase A and B: their role in the degradation of serotonin. In:Baumgarten HG, Gothert M, editors. 5-HT Neurons and 5-HT Receptors in the CNS. Handbook Exp Pharmacol v.129. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. 1997, 655-671.
    [33] Shih JC, Chen K, Ridd MJ. Monoamine oxidase: from genes to behavior. Ann Rev Neurosci 1999,22:197-217.
    [34] Fitzpatrick PF. Tetrahydropterin-dependent amino acid hydroxylases.Ann Rev Biochem, 1999,68:355-381.
    [35] Darmon MC, Guibert B, Leviel V, Ehret M, Maitre M, et al.Sequence of two mRNAs encoding active rat tryptophan hydroxylase.J Neurochem, 1988, 51:312-316.
    [36] Stoll J, Kozak CA, Goldman D. Characterization and chromosomal mapping of a cDNA encoding TPH from a mouse mastocytoma cell line.Genomics ,1990. 7:88-96.
    [37] Boularand S, Darmon MC, Ganem Y, Malet J. Complete coding sequence of human TPH. Nucleic Acids Res,1990.18:42-57.
    [38] Walther DJ, Peter JU, Bashammakh S, Hortnagl H, Voits M, et al.Synthesis of serotonin by a second TPH isoform. Science,2003.299:76.
    [39] Walther DJ, Bader M. A unique central tryptophan hydroxylase isoform. Biochem Pharmacol, 2003.66:1673-1680.
    [40] Zhang X, Beaulieu JM, Sotnikova TD, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG. TPH-2 controls brain serotonin synthesis. Science ,2004.305:217.
    [41] Kulikov AV, Osipova DV, Naumenko VS, Popova NK. Association between Tph2 gene polymorphism, brain tryptophan hydroxylase activity and aggressiveness in mouse strains. Genes, Brain and Behavior,2005.4:482—485.
    [42] Veenstra-Vander Weele J,Cook EH Jr.Knockout mouse points to second form of tryptophan hydroxylase.Mol Interv.2003;3(2):72-75.
    [43] Walther DJ,Peter JU,Bashammakh S,et al.Synthesis of serotonin by a second tryptophan hydroxylase isoform.Science.2003:299(5603):76
    [44] Auerbach JG, Faroy M, Ebstein R, et al.The association of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) and the serotonin transporter promoter gene (5-HTTLPR) with temperament in 12-month-old infants.J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 2001:42:777-783.
    [45] Holmes A, Murphy DL, Crawley JN. Reduced aggression in mice lacking the serotonin transporter. Psychopharmacology. 2002,161(2):160-167.
    [46] Brunner HG ,Nelen M, Breakefield XO et al. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science. 1993 22;262(5133):578-580.
    [47]Nelson RJ, Chiavegatto S. Molecular basis of aggression.Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(12):713-719
    [48] Manuck SB, Flory JD, Ferrell RE, et al. A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry Res. 2000,24(1):9-23.
    [49]Foley DL, Eaves LJ, Wormley B, et al. Childhood adversity, monoamine oxidase a genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004 , 61(7):738-744.
    [50] Bach AW, Lan NC, Johnson DL, Abell CW, Bembenek ME, et al. cDNA cloning of human liver monoamine oxidase A and B: molecular basis of differences in enzymatic properties. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 1988.85:4934-4938.
    [51] Lan NC, Heizmann C, Gal H, Klisak I, Orth U, et al. Human monoamine oxidase A and B genes map to Xp11.23 and are deleted in patients with Norrie disease. Genomics, 1989.4:552-559.
    [52] Cases O, Seif I, Grimsby J, Gaspar P, Chen K, et al. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science, 1995.268:1763-1766.
    [53] De Maeyer E, Self I, Cases O, Gaspar P. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain 5-HT in mice lacking monoamine oxidase A. In: Grisolia JS, et al. editors. Violence: From Biology to Society. Amsterdam: Elsevier Sci. 1997;71-78.
    [54] Popova NK, Gilinsky MA, Amstislavskaya TG, Morosova EA, Seif I, et al.Regional 5-HT metabolism in the brain of transgenic mice lacking monoamine oxidase A. J Neurosci Res 2001.66:423-427.
    [55] Evrard A, Malagie I, Laprte A-M, Boni C, Hanoun N, et al. Altered regulation of the 5-HT system in the brain of MAO-A knockout mice. Eur J Neurosci 2002.15:841-851.
    [56] Shih JC, Chen K, Ridd MJ. Monoamine oxidase: from genes to behavior. Ann Rev Neurosci, 1999.22:197-217.
    [57] Brunner HG. Monoamine oxidase and behavior. Ann Med 1995.27:431-432.
    [58] Samochowiec J, Lesch KP, Rottmann M, Smolka M, et al. Association of a regulatory polymorphism in the prompter region of the monoamine oxidase A gene with antisocial alcoholism. Psychiatry Res,1999.86:67-72.
    [59] Xiao PF, Cheng L,Wan Y, et al. An improved gel-based DNA microarray method for detecting single nucleotide mismatch, Electrophoresis. 2006,27(19): 3904-3915
    [60] Ainslie, G Spelious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull 1975,128:463-496
    [61] Metcalfe J. Mischel W. A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification:dynamics of widpower, Psychological Bulletin, 1999,106:3-19
    [62] Gottfredon MR, Hirschi T.General theory of crime. Stanford CA:Stanford Vniversity press, 1990.
    [63] Hinslie L, Shatzky J, Psychiatric Dictionary,New York, Oxford university press,1940
    [64] Moeller FG Barrat, ES. Dougherty DM, et al Psychiatric Aspects of impulsivity, Am J psychintry, 2001,156:1783-1793
    [65] Tusing ME, Ford L. Examining preschool cognitive abilities using a CHC framework. International Journal of Testing,2004, 4: 91-114
    [66] Flanagan DP, McGrew KS. Interpreting intelligence tests from contemporary Gf- Gc theory: Joint confirmatory factor analysis of the WJ- R and KAIT in a non- White sample. Journal of School Psychology, 1998, 36: 151- 182
    [67] Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt Factor structure of twe barratt Impulsiveness scale, J Clin psychol,1995,51:768-774
    [68] 吴明隆编著..SPSS统计应用实务.第Ⅰ版北京:中国铁道出版社.,2000.
    [69] Cole DA. Utility of confirmatory factor analysis in test validation research.J con- suit Clin Psychol, 1987:55(4):584-594.
    [70] Manne S, Schnoll R. Measuring cancer patient's psychological d istress and well- being: A factor analytic assessment of the Mental Health Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 2001,13 (1): 99-109.
    [71] Fan X, Wang L. Effects of potential confounding factors on fit indices and parameter estimates for true and misspecified SEM models. Educational and Psychological Measurement,1998,58: 701-735
    [72] Cox BJ, Enns MW, Clam IP. The multidimensional structure of perfectionism in clinically distressed and college student samples. Psychological Assessment,2002, 14(3):365^373
    [73] Bischkopf J, Busse A, Angermeyer MC. Mild cognitive impairment: A review of prevalence, incidence and outcome according to current approaches. Acta Psychiatr Scand, 2002,106(6):403-404.
    [74] 岳冬梅.心理卫生评定量表手册 北京:中国心理卫生杂志社出版,1999.161-167.
    [75] McDevitt, SC.&Carey, WB.et al.The measurement of temperament in 3-7 years old children. J-Child-Psychol-Psychiaty. 1978,19:245-253.
    [76] 洪琦,张风,周胜利等.儿童气质测查量表的修订和标准化,中华儿科杂志,1999,37(3) 135-138
    [77] Hegvik RL,McDevitt, SC.,Carey WB.The middle childhood temperament questionnaire.Journal of development and behavioral Pediatrics.1982, (4):197-200
    [78] 苏林雁,罗学荣,张纪水,等.儿童自我意识量表的中国城市常模中国心理卫生杂志 2002,16(1):31-34
    [79] 中国心理卫生杂志社主编.心理卫生评定量表手册.北京:中国心理卫生杂志社出版,1999.52-420
    [80] Barkiey RA. Hypenactive children: a handbook for diagnosis treatment[M]. The 2nd. New york: The Guiford press. 1982. 132-134.
    [81] 刘培毅,何慕陶.心理卫生评定量表手册.北京 中国心理卫生杂志社出版,1999.149-152.
    [82] Shaw DS, Owens EB, Giovannelli J, et al.Infant and toddler pathways leading to early externalizing disorders[J].J-Am-Acad-Child-Adolesc-Psychiatry, 2001 Jan, 40(1):36-43
    [83] Sheeber L, Sorensen E.Family relationship of depressed adolescents: a multi-method assessment[J].J Clin Chlid Psychol, 1998, 27 (3): 268-277
    [84] Gjone H.The association between internalizing and externalizing behavior in childhoodand early adolescence: genetic of environmental common influences ? [J].JAbnorm Child Psychol, 1997, 25 (4): 277-286
    [85] 张丽华.父母教养方式与儿童社会化发展研究综述,过宁师大学报(社会科学版),1997,3:19-22.
    [86] 岳冬梅:父母养育方式评定量表.心理卫生评定量表册(增定版).中国心理卫生杂志社,1999.161-167
    [87] 高雪屏,于素维,苏林雁,儿童青少年多动冲动行为与父母养育方式的关系,中国临床心理学杂志 2003年 第11卷 第1期 36-38
    [88] Cunningham CE,Barkley RA.The interactions of normal and hyperactive children and their mothers in free play and structured tasks.Child Dev,1979,50:217-224
    [89] Mash EJ,Johnston C.A comparison of the mother-child interactions of younger and older hyperactive and normal children.Child Dev, 1982,53:1371-81
    [90] Prinzie P, Swillen A, Maes B, et al. Parenting, family contexts, and personality characteristics in youngsters with VCFS.Genet Couns,2004,15 (2): 141-157.
    [91] Weiss B, Dodge KA, Bates JE, et al. Some consequences of early harsh discipline: child aggression and a maladaptive social information processing style. Child Dev, 1992,63(6): 1321-1325.
    [92] 傅晓燕,孟馥,梅竹,张旭.注意缺陷多动障碍儿童气质特征及心理治疗,同济大学学报(医学版).2005,26(1)82-84
    [93] Stella SMD.Origin and evolution of behavior disorders form infance to early adult life [M].New York: Brunner/Mazel Press,1984.
    [94] 洪琦,姚凯南,刘灵.8-12岁儿童气质量表的修订和标准化[J].中华儿童保健杂志,1996,4(4):128-131.
    [95] 张劲松,王玉凤.儿童气质与行为问题关系的研究[J].中国心理卫生杂志,1996,10(6) 248-252.
    [96] Prior M,Smart D.Sex differences in psychological adjustmem from infancy to 8 years [J]. J AM Acad Child A dilese Psychiatry,1993,32(2)287-290.
    [97] Thomas A,Chess S.The reality of difficult remperament [J].Merriu Palmer Quality,1982,28 (1) 1-19.
    [98] Mische IW.Introduction to Personality. Holt:Pine hartand Winston.1999. 315-342.
    [99] 王玲凤.学龄儿童的行为问题与自我概念研究.中国学校卫生,2004,25(2):162.
    [100] 黄广文.苏林雁.苏巧荣.任颖,对立违抗性障碍儿童的自我意识及家庭环境因素分析.中国神经精神疾病杂志,2006,32(5):403-406
    [101] 李方,阎斌,梁志中等.学习困难儿童的心理行为特征.中国神经精神疾病杂 志,2003,29(5):383.
    [102] Baumeister RF. Self-Esteem-the puzzle of low Self-Regard.Plenium Press New York,1993.
    [103] Schneider M,Leithenbert H.A comparison of aggressive and withdraw children's self-esteem : optimism and pessimism , and causal attributions for success and failure.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,1989,17:133-144
    [104] Lochman J, LampronL.Situational social problem-solving skills and self-esteem of aggressive and nonaggres-sive boys Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,1986,14:605-617
    [105] Savin-Williams,Jaquish.The assessment of adolescent self-esteem : acomparison of methods , Journal of Personality,1981,49:324-336
    [106] Crocker J, Park LE.The costly pursuit of self-esteem , Psychological Bulletin , 2004,130:392-414
    [107] Epstein NB et al. The McMaster family assessment device. J of Marital among Family Therapy, 1983,9(2):171-180.
    [108] Olson DH. Circumplex model of marital and family systems : Assessing family functioning In : Walsh Feds . Normal family process 2~(nd) ed . New York : The Guilford Press ,1993 : 109 -133.
    [109] Sherry ST, Ward MH, Kholodov M et al.dbSNP: the NCBI database of genetic variation.Nucleic Acids Res. 2001,29(1):308-311.
    [110] Fredman D, Siegfried M, Yuan YP et al. HGVbase: a human sequence variation database emphasizing data quality and a broad spectrum of data sources. Nucleic Acids Res. 2002,7,30(1):387-391.
    [111] Coccaro EF, Lawrence T, Trestman R et al. Growth hormone responses to intravenous clonidine challenge correlate with behavioral irritability in psychiatric patients and healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Res,1991, 39(2): 129-139
    [112] Sachidanandam R, Weissman D, Schmidt SC et al. A map of human genome sequence variation containing 1.42 million single nucleotide polymer- phisms.Nature.2001,409(6822):928-933.
    [113] Lander ES. The new genomics: global views of biology. Science. 1996,25;274(5287):536-539
    [114] David Huizinga, Brett C. Haerstic k, et al. Subsequent Antisocial Behavior, and the Role of Monoamine Oxidase Agenotype, Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:677-683
    [115] http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/SNP/snpref.
    [116] Heisler LK, Chu H-M, Brennan TJ, Danao JA, Bajwa R, et al.Elevated anxiety and antidepressant-like responses in serotonin 5-HT1A receptor mutant mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci, 1998.95:15049-15054.
    [117] Pineyro G, Blier P. Autoregulation of serotonin neurons: role in antidepressant drug action. Pharmacol Rews, 1999.51:533-591.
    [118] Pruus K, Skrebuhhova-Malmros T, Rudissaar R, Matto V, Allikmets L.5-HT1A receptor agonists buspirone and gepirone attenuate apomorphine-induced aggressive behavior in adult male Wistar rats.J Physiol Pharmacol , 2002.51:833-846.
    [119] Nelson RJ, Chiavegatto S. Molecular basis of aggression.Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(12):7I3-719
    [120] Manuck SB, Flory JD, Ferrell RE, et al. A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry Res. 2000,24(1):9-23.
    [121] Veenstra-VanderWeele J,Cook EH Jr.Knockout mouse points to second form of tryptophan hydroxylase.Mol Interv.2003;3(2):72-75.
    [122] Walther DJ,Peter JU,Bashammakh S,et al.Synthesis of serotonin by a second tryptophan hydroxylase isoform.Science.2003:299(5603):76
    [123] Walitza S , Renner TJ , Dempfle A , et al. Transmission disequilibrium of polymorphic variants in the tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene in attention-deflcit/hyperactivity disorder, Molecular Psychiatry (2005), 1-7
    [124] Van Den Bogaert A, V Sleegers K, De Zutter S, Heyrman L, et al. Association of brain-specific tryptophan hydroxylase, TPH2, with unipolar and bipolar disorder in a Northern Swedish, isolated population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006 Oct;63(10):1103-1110.
    [125] Sacco R, Papaleo V, Hager J, etal. Case-control and family-based association studies of candidate genes in autistic disorder and its endophenotypes: TPH2 and GLO1. BMC Med Genet. 2007 Mar 8;8(1):11
    [126] Gutknecht L, Jacob C, Strobel A, et al.Tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variation influences personality traits and disorders related to emotional dysregulation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006, 19: 1-12
    [127] Reuter M, Kuepper Y, Hennig J. Association between a polymorphism in the promoter region of the TPH2 gene and the personality trait of harm avoidance. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006, 19:1-4
    [128] Mossner R, Walitza S, Geller F, et al. Transmission disequilibrium of polymorphic variants in the tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006 ;9(4):437-442.
    [129] Walitza S, Renner TJ, Dempfle A, et al. Transmission disequilibrium of polymorphic variants in the tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene in attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2005 Dec;10(12):1126-1132.
    
    [130] http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/SNP/snp_ref.cgi?rs=4570625
    [131] Retz W, Retz-Junginger P, Supprian T, Thome J, Rosier M.Association of serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism with violence: relation with personality disorders, impulsivity, and childhood ADHD psychopathology. Behav Sci Law 2004; 22:415-2560.
    [132] Zill P, Baghai TC, Zwanzger P, Schule C, Eser D, Rupprecht R et al.SNP and haplotype analysis of a novel tryptophan hydroxylase isoform (TPH2) gene provide evidece for association with major depression. Mol Psychiatry 2004; 9: 1030-1036.
    [133] Harvey M, Shink E, Tremblay M, Gagne B, Raymond C, Labbe M et al. Support for the involvement of TPH2 gene in affective disorders. Mol Psychiatry 2004; 9: 980-981.
    [134] Breidenthal SE, White DJ, Glatt CE. Identification of genetic variants in the neuronal form of tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH2).Psychiatr Genet 2004; 14: 69-72.
    [135] Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 1996; 274:1527-1531.
    [136] Maron E, Toru I, Must A,et al. Association study of tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene polymorphisms in panic disorder, Neurosci Lett. 2007 16;411(3): 180-1844.
    [137] Courtet P, Baud P, Abbar M, et al.Association between violent suicidal behavior and the low activity allele of the serotonin transporter gene. Mol Psychiatry, 2001, 6(3): 338-341.
    [138] Liao DL, Hong CJ, Shih HL,et al.Possible association between serotonin transporter promoter region polymorphism and extremely violent crime in Chinese males. Neuropsychobiology,2004,50(4):284 287.
    [139] Danielle LS,Pollock, BG, Robert AS,et al. The 5-HTTPR*S/*L Polymorphism and Aggressive Behavior in Alzheimer Disease.Arch Neurol,2001, 58(9): 1425-1428.
    [140] Manuck SB, Flory JD, Muldoon MF,et al. Central nervous system sero- tonergic responsivity and aggressive disposition in men. Physiol Behav, 2002, 77 (4-5):705-709.
    [141] Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE,et al.Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science,2002,297(5582):851 — 854.
    [142] Vanyukov MM,Moss HB,Yu LM,Deka R A dinucleotide repeat polymorphism at the gene for monoamine oxidase A and measures of aggressiveness. Psychiatry Res,1995,59(1-2):35--41.
    [143] Koller G, Bondy B, Preuss UW,et al. No association between a polymorphism in the promoter region of the MAOA gene with antisocial personality traits in alcoholics. Alcohol Alcohol,2003,38(1):31 -34.
    [144] Zanunit S, Jones G, Jones SJ, et al. Polymorphisms in the MAOA, MAOB, and COMT genes and aggressive behavior in schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet,2004, 128B(1):19-20.
    [145] Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D.A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter.Hum Genet, 1998,103(3):273-279.
    [1] Moyer KE. Kinds of aggression and their physiological basis.Comm Behav Biol, 1968.2:65-87.
    [2] Ainslie, G Spelious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull 1975,128:463-496
    [3] Gottfredon MR, Hirschi T.General theory of crime. Stanford CA:Stanford Vniversity press, 1990.
    [4] Metcalfe J. Mischel W. A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of widpower, Psychological Bulletin, 1999,106:3-19
    [5] Hinslie L, Shatzky J, Psychiatric Dictionary ,New York , Oxford university press, 1940
    [6] Moeller FG Barrat, ES. Dougherty DM,et al Psychiatric Aspects of impulsivity, Am J psychintry, 2001,156:1783-1793
    [7] Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt Factor structure of twe barratt Impulsiveness scale, J Clin psychol,1995,51:768-774
    [8] Carolyn WS,M.Jamila R, Mary H.Treating children with early-onset condust problems:Intervention outcomes for parent,child,and teacher training.Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,2004:33 (1): 105-124.
    [9] David E C, Radhika GA, Nancy G,et al.Comparison of the role of dopamine,serotonin, and noradrenaline genes in ADHD, ODD and conduct disorder:multivariate regression analysis of 20 genes. Clin Genet,2000: 57: 178-196
    [10] Yuzuru H, Tohru Y, Kazuhiko S. Psychosocial problems in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with oppositional defiant disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences,2002:56:365-369
    [11] Maxson SC. Aggression: Concepts and methods relevant to genetic analysis in mice and humans. In: Jones BC, Mormede P, editors.Neurobehavioral Genetics. Methods and Applications. Boca Raton: CRC Press. 1999; 293-300.
    [12] World Report on Violence and Health. World Health Organization.Geneva, Switzerland: 2002. 340.
    [13] MaxsonSC. Potentialgeneticmodelsofaggressionandviolencein males. In: Driscoll P, editor. Genetically Defined Animal Models of Neurobehavioral Dysfunctions. Boston: Birkhauser. 1992; 174-188.
    [14] Plomin R, Owen MJ, McGuffin P. The genetic basis of complex human behaviors. Science 1994, 264:1733-1739.
    [15] Reif A, Lesch K-P. Toward a molecular architecture of personality. Behav Brain Res 2003, 139:1-20.
    [16] Peterson J, Guyer M, Felsenfeld A, Old S. Genome sequence of the Brown Norway rat yields insights into mammalian evolution. Nature 2004. 428 (1 Apr):493-521.
    [17] Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. Inadequacy of pain-aggression hypothesis in naturalistic settings. Aggress Behav, 1990.10:33-46.18. Belyaev DK.
    [18] Destabilizing selection as a factor of domestication.J Heredity, 1979.70:301-308.
    [19] Crowe RR, An adoption of antisocial personality,Arch Gen Psychiatry, 1974,31:785-791
    [20] Coccaro EF, Bergeman CS, Kavoussi RJ,etc. Heritability of aggression and irritability:a twin study of the Buss-Durkee aggression scales in adult male subjects,Biol Psychiatry, 1997,41:273-284
    [21] Brunner HG, Nelen M, Breakefield XO, Ropers HH, Van Oost BA.Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science, 1993.262:578-580.
    [22] Brunner HG. Monoamine oxidase and behavior. Ann Med, 1995.27:431-432.
    [23] Samochowiec J, Lesch KP, Rottmann M, Smolka M, et al.Association of a regulatory polymorphism in the prompter region of the monoamine oxidase A gene with antisocial alcoholism. Psychiatry Res, 1999.86:67-72.
    [24] Castellanos FX, EliaJ, KruesiMJ, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid monoamine metabolites in boys with attention deficithy peractivity disorder. PsychiatryRes, 1994,52:305-316.
    [25] 苏林雁,谢光荣,高雪屏,等.注意缺陷多动障碍合并对立违抗性障碍患儿血清5-羟色胺的对照研究)[J].中华精神科杂志,2001,34(4):230
    [26] Olivier, B. et al. Serotonin receptors and animal models of aggressive behavior.Pharmacopsychiatry, 1995,28, 80-90
    [27] Miczek, K.A. et al. Alcohol-heightened aggression in mice: attenuation by 5-HT1A receptor agonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl.), 1998,139, 160-168
    [28] Fish, E.W. et al. Aggression heightened by alcohol or social instigation in mice: reduction by the 5-HT(1B) receptor agonist CP-94,253.Psychopharmacology (Berl),1999,146, 391-399
    [29] Lyons, W.E. et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor-deficient mice develop aggressiveness and hyperphagia in conjunction with brain serotonergic abnormalities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.,1999,96, 15239-15244
    [30] Chiavegatto, S. et al. Brain serotonin dysfunction accounts for aggression in male micelacking neuronal nitric oxide synthase. Proc. Natl.Acad. Sci. U. S. A.,2001,98, 1277-1281
    [31] Popova NK, Naumenko VS, Plyusnina IZ, Kulikov AV. Reduction in 5-HT1A receptor density, 5-HT1A mRNA expression, and functional correlates for 5-HT1A receptors in genetically defined aggressive rats. J Neurosci Res,2005.80:286-292.
    [32] 赵汉清,端义扬,刘桂永.5-经色胺异常在自杀行为中的作用,山东精神医学 2004,17(1):54-56
    [33] Holmes A,Murphy DL,Cra wle y J N.Redyced aggression in mice lacking the serotonin transporter.Psychopharmacology.2002,16(2): 160-167
    [34] Higley JD,Linnoila M.Low central nervous system serotonergic activity is traiflike and correlaces with impulsive be havior:a nonhuman Primate model neurotrans- mission investigating genetic and environmental influences on Ann N Y Acad sci,1997,836:39-56.
    [35] Cook EH,Fletcher KE,Wainwright M,etc.Primary structure of the human platelet serotonin 5-HT-2A receptor,Journal of Neurochemistry,1994,63:465-469
    [36] Brown GL,Linnoila MI. CSF Serotonin Metabolism Studies in Depression, Impulsivity, and Violence.) Clin Psychiatry, 1990, 51 (suppl 4): 31-41
    [37] Royce Lee,Coccaro E. The Neuropsychopharmacology of Criminality and Aggression, Can J Psychiatry,2001, 46:35-44
    [38] Virkunen M, Rawling R,Tokola R,etc. CSF Biochemistries, Glucose Metabolism, and Diurnal Activity Rhythms in Alcoholic, Violent Offenders, Fire Setters, and Healthy Volunteers[)], Arch Gen Psychiatry, 1994, 51:20-27
    [39] Evenden JL.Varieties of impulsivity.Psychophacology,1999,146:348-361
    [40] Strayhorn JM Jr. Self-control: theory and research..J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002 Jan;41(1):7-16.
    [41] Auerbach JG, Faroy M, Ebstein R, et al.The association of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) and the serotonin transporter promoter gene (5-HTTLPR) with temperament in 12-month-old infants. J Child Psychol Psychiatry,2001:42:777-783.
    [42] Downey KK, Stelson FW, Pomerleau OF, et al. Adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: psychological test profiles in a clinical population. J Nerv Ment Dis, 1997:185:32-38.
    [43] Ruchkin W, Eisemann M, Cloninger CR. Behaviour/emotional problems in male juvenile delinquents and controls in Russia: the role of personality traits. Acta Psychiatr Scand, 1998 :98:231-236.
    [44] Linnoila M, Virkkunen M, Scheman M, et. al. Cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentes impulsive from nonimpilsive vionlent behavior.,Life Sci, 1983,33:2609-2614
    [45] Dalley JW, Fryer TD, Brichard L, et al. Rats that tend to abuse cocaine have lower dopamine receptor availability even before drug exposure, suggesting that this trait is preexisting and not a result of drug abuse. Science, 2007: 1267-1270.
    [46] Brown GL,Goodwin FK, Ballenger JC,etc.Aggression in humans correlates with cerebrospinal fluid amine metabolites,Psychiatry Res, 1979, 1 (2) :131-139.
    [47] Virkkunen M,Nuutila A,Goodwin FK,etc. Cerebrospinal fluid monoamine metabolite levels in male arsonists, Arch Gen Psychiatry, 1997, 44:241-247
    [48] Gregg,T.R.and Siegel,A.Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: implications for human aggression. Prog. Neuro- Psychopharmacol. Biol.Psychiatry,2001, 25:91—140
    [49] Hall, M.E. and Stewart, J.M. Modulation of isolation-induced fighting by N- and C-terminal analogs of substance P: evidence for multiple recognition sites. Peptides, 1984, 5:85-89
    [50] File, S.E. NKP609, an NK1 receptor antagonist, has an anxiolytic action in the social interaction test in rats. Psychopharmacology,2000,152:105-109
    
    [51] Nelson, R.J. et al. Role of nitric oxide in neuroendocrine regulation of physiology and behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 1997,18:463-491
    [52] Simon, N.G. et al. Testosterone and its metabolites modulate 5HT1A and 5HT1B agonist effects on intermale aggression. Neurosci.Biobehav. Rev, 1998, 23, 325-336
    [53] Dolan M, Anderson IM, Deakin JF, Relationship between 5-HT function and impulsivity and aggression in male offenders with personality disorders, Br J Psychiatry, 2001,178:352-359
    [54] Sumner, B.E. and Fink, G. Testosterone as well as estrogen increases serotonin 2A receptor mRNA and binding site densities in the male rat brain. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res, 1998, 59,205-214
    [55] Ferrari, P.F. et al. The influence of gender and age on neonatal rat hypothalamic 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors. Cell Mol. Neurobiol, 1999, 19,775-784
    [56] Zhang, L. et al. Sex differences in expression of serotonin receptors (subtypes 1A and 2A) in rat brain: Apossible role of testosterone. Neuroscience, 1999, 94, 251-259
    [57] Ferris, C.F. et al. Serotonin regulation of aggressive behavior in male golden hamsters(Mesocricetus auratus). Behav Neurosci, 1999, 113,804-815
    [58] Agren H, Mefford IN, Rudorfer MV, etc. Intercating neuroransmitter systems:A non-experimental approach to the 5-HIAA correlation in human CSF,J Psychiatr Res,1986,20:175-193
    [59] Brodkin ES, Goforth SA, Keene AH, Fosella JA, Silver LM.Identification of quantitative trait loci that affect aggressive behavior in mice. J Neurosci, 2000, 22:1165-1170.
    [60] Whitaker-Azmitia PM, Druse M, Walker P, Lauder JM. Serotonin as a developmental signal. Behav Brain Res, 1996, 73:19-29.
    [61] Popova NK. Brain serotonin in genetically defined defensive behavior. In: Millar R, Ivanitsky AM, Balaban PM, editors. Complex Brain Functions: Conceptual Advances in Russian Neuroscience. London:Harwood Press, 1999.317-329.
    [62] Linnoila VM, Virkkunen M. Aggression, suicidality, and serotonin.J Clin Psychiatry, 1992.53:46-51.
    
    [63] Molina V, Ciesielski L, Gobaille S, Isel F, Mandel P. Inhibition of mouse-killing behavior by serotonin-mimetic drugs: effects of partial alterations of serotonin neurotransmission. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 1987. 27:123-131.
    [64] Arango V, Huang Y, Underwood MD, Mann JJ. Genetics of the serotonin system in suicidal behavior. J Psychiat Res, 2003.37:375-386.
    [65] Nelson RJ, Chiavegatto S. Molecular basis of aggression.Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(12):713-719
    [66] Manuck SB, Flory JD, Ferrell RE, et al. A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry Res. 2000,24(1):9-23.
    [67] Veenstra-VanderWeele J,Cook EH Jr.Knockout mouse points to second form of tryptophan hydroxylase.Mol Interv.2003;3(2):72-75.
    [68] Walther DJ,Peter JU,Bashammakh S,et al.Synthesis of serotonin by a second tryptophan hydroxylase isoform.Science.2003:299(5603):76
    [69] Kulikov AV, Osipova DV, Naumenko VS, Popova NK. Association between Tph2 gene polymorphism, brain tryptophan hydroxylase activity and aggressiveness in mouse strains. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 2005.4:482-485.
    [70] Gainetdinov RR, Sotnikova TD, Caron MG. Monoamine transporter pharmacology and mutant mice. Trends Pharmacol Sci , 2002.23:367-373.
     [71] Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG. Monoamine transporter: from genes to behavior. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol , 2003.43:261-284.
    [72] Auerbach JG, Faroy M, Ebstein R, et al.The association of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) and the serotonin transporter promoter gene (5-HTTLPR) with temperament in 12-month-old infants.J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 2001:42:777-783.
    [73] Frisch A, Finkel B, Michaelovsky E. A rare short allele of the serotonin transporter promoter region (5-HTTLPR) found in an aggressive schiz-ophrenic patient of Jewish Libyan origin. Psychiatr Genet,2000,10(4): 179-183.
    [74] Holmes A, Murphy DL, Crawley JN. Reduced aggression in mice lacking the serotonin transporter. Psychopharmacology. 2002 ,161(2): 160-167.
    [75] Courtet P, Baud P, Abbar M, et al.Association between violent suicidal behavior and the low activity allele of the serotonin transporter gene. Mol Psychiatry,2001, 6(3): 338-341.
    [76] Evans J, Reeves B, Platt H, et al. Impulsiveness, serotonin genes and repetition of deliberate self-harm (DSH).Psychological Medicine,2000, 30(6):1327-1334.
    [77] Danielle LS,Pollock, BG, Robert AS,et al. The 5-HTTPR*S/*L Polymorphism and Aggressive Behavior in Alzheimer Disease. Arch Neurol,2001, 58(9): 1425-1428.
    [78] Kotler M, Barak P, Cohen H, et al. Homicidal behavior in schizophrenia associated with a genetic polymorphism determining low catechol-o- methyltransferase activity. Am J Med Genet, 1999,88 (6):628-633.
    [79] Heisler LK, Chu H-M, Brennan TJ, Danao JA, Bajwa R, et al.Elevated anxiety and antidepressant-like responses in serotonin 5-HT1A receptor mutant mice. ProcNatlAcad Sci, 1998.95:15049-15054.
    [80] Pineyro G, Blier P. Autoregulation of serotonin neurons: role in antidepressant drug action. Pharmacol Rews, 1999.51:533-591.
    [81] Pruus K, Skrebuhhova-Malmros T, Rudissaar R, Matto V, Allikmets L.5-HT1A receptor agonists buspirone and gepirone attenuate apomorphine-induced aggressive behavior in adult male Wistar rats.J Physiol Pharmacol , 2002.51:833-846.
    [82] Cleare AJ, Bond AJ. Ipsapirone challenge in aggressive men shows an inverse correlation between 5-HT1A receptor function and aggression. Psychop- harmacology, 2000.148:344-349.
    [83] Parsey RV, Oguendo MA, Simpson NR, Ogden RT, Van Heertum R, etal.Effect of sex, age, and aggressive traits in man on brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding potential measured by PET using [C-11]WAY-00635. Brain Res, 2002.954:173-182.
    [84] Lai MK, Tsang SW, Francis PT, Esiri MM, Keene MM, et al. Reduced serotonin 5-HT2A receptor binding in the temporal cortex correlates withaggressivebehavior inAlzheimerdisease.BrainRes, 2003.974:82-87.
    [85] Popova NK, Gilinsky MA, Amstislavskaya TG, Morosova EA, Seif I, et al.Regional 5-HT metabolism in the brain of transgenic mice lacking monoamine oxidase A. J Neurosci Res, 2001.66:423-427.
    [86] Cases O, Seif I, Grimsby J, Gaspar P, Chen K, et al. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science, 1995.268:1763-1766.
    [87] Popova NK, Gilinsky MA, Amstislavskaya TG, Morosova EA, Seif I, et al. Regional 5-HT metabolism in the brain of transgenic mice lacking monoamine oxidase A. J Neurosci Res, 2001.66:423-427.
    [88] Evrard A, Malagie I, Laprte A-M, Boni C, Hanoun N, et al. Altered regulation ofthe5-HT systeminthebrain ofMAO-A knockout mice. EurJ Neurosci , 2002.15:841-851.
    [89] Shih JC, Chen K, Ridd MJ. Monoamine oxidase: from genes to behavior. Ann Rev Neurosci, 1999.22:197-217.
    [90] Brunner HG ,Nelen M, Breakefield XO et al. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science.1993 22;262(5133):578-580.
    [91] Nelson RJ, Chiavegatto S. Molecular basis of aggression.Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(12):713-719
    [92] Manuck SB, Flory JD, Ferrell RE, et al. A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry Res.2000,24(1):9-23.
    [93] Foley DL, Eaves LJ, Wormley B, et al. Childhood adversity, monoamine oxidase a genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004 , 61(7):738-744.
    [94] Manor I, Tyano S, Eisenberg J, et al. The short DRD4 repeats confer risk to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a family-based design and impair performance on a continuous performance test (TOVA). Mol Psychiatry, 2002:7:790-794.
    
    [95] Evenden JL.Varieties of impulsivity.Psychophacology,1999 146:348-361
    
    [96] Rowe DC, Stever C, Chase D, er al. Two dopamine genes related to reports of childhood retrospective inattention and conduct disorder symptoms. Mol Psychiatry, 2001:6:429-433.
    [97] Comings DE, Chen C, Wu S,et al. Association of the androgen receptor gene(AR) with ADHD and conduct disorder. Neuroreport,1999,10(7):1589-1592.
    
    [98] Isabelle L,Stephanie P,Stephane M,et al.Loss of Aggression, After Transfer onto a C57BL16J Background, in Mice Carrying a Targeted Disruption of the Neuronal Nitric Oxide Synthase.Gene Behavior Genetics, 2000,30(5):367-373.
    [99] Du L, Bakish D, Hrdina PD. Gender difference in association between serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and personality traits. Psychiatr Genet,2000, (4): 159-164
    [100] Sher L, Greenberg BD, Murphy DL, et al .Pleiotropy of the serotion transporter gene for seasonality and neuroticism_ Psychiatr Genet, 2000,10(3): 125-130.
    [101] Vander Vegt BJ,Lieuwes N,vander Wall EH,Kato K,Moya-AlbiolL, etal.Activation of serotonergic neurotransmission during the perfor-mance of aggressive behavior in rats. Behav Neurosci, 2003.117:667-674.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700