用户名: 密码: 验证码:
自我相关信息对知觉选择的调控
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
已有大量研究表明个人重要信息调控高级认知加工过程,如面孔识别、记忆、思维等。本研究利用四个研究系统地测量了个人重要信息对知觉选择的调控作用及其神经机制。研究采用最近发展的自我连接学习范式,让中性几何图形与不同人(自我、朋友、陌生人)建立联系;图形-标签连接完成后,以复合图形(局部小图形组成整体大图形)作为实验刺激,被试完成整体/局部图形判断任务,通过评估个体对具有不同社会意义图形(目标vs.分心物水平)的感知差异,测量个人重要信息对注意选择的调控作用。
     研究一采用三个行为实验,分别利用集中注意或分散注意任务,系统地考察自我相关信息作为目标和作为分心物两种条件下,其对整体优先效应的调控。结果发现,与他人相关图形相比,自我相关图形作为分心物调控整体优先效应,这种效应一致地发生在整体和局部水平上,并且不受注意任务的影响。这些行为结果提示社会信息对认知的调控作用发生在早期视觉选择水平上。
     研究二采用两个行为实验,直接比较自我相关信息在两个不同注意任务下对整体优先效应的调控作用的差异(集中注意和分散注意作为被试间变量)。结果发现,当社会信息作为分心物出现的时候,自我相关信息对知觉选择的调控在分散注意任务大于集中注意任务条件。结果表明社会信息对注意策略的调控作用。
     研究三利用事件相关电位技术,结合自我相关信息在分散注意任务产生较大调控作用的实验结果,考察这种效应是否发生在知觉选择的早期阶段。结果发现,在分散注意任务中,当社会信息作为分心物的时候,与前人研究一致:整体与局部水平的加工差异发生在P1和N1早成分上,而自我对整体/局部加工的调控发生主要发生在N1成分上。这表明自我相关信息对知觉选择的调控发生在视觉早期感知加工过程中。
     研究四采用功能性核磁共振(fMRI)方法记录在分散注意条件下,自我相关信息对知觉选择的调控时大脑活动的变化。结果发现,当自我信息作为分心物出现的时候,内侧前额叶(自我加工脑区)的激活大于朋友作为分心物的时候;朋友作为分心物出现的时候,加工整体与加工局部水平引起左侧顶内沟(注意加工脑区)活动的差异,而并未在自我作为分心物的条件下发现;自我作为目标出现的时候,比朋友作为目标的条件下引发右侧顶内沟更高的激活。结果表明,自我相关信息对知觉选择的调控受到自我相关的神经机制——内侧前额叶皮层以及对突显信息注意机制——双侧顶内沟的调节。综上所述,研究采用四个研究七个实验分别从行为,脑电,以及fMRI三个方面考察自我相关信息对知觉选择的调控作用。结果表明自我相关信息能够对知觉选择起到调控的作用。并且自我相关信息对知觉选择的调控在集中与分散两种注意方式上类似,但是在分散注意中影响程度更大。这一调控过程发生在初级感知阶段,并且受到自我网络和注意网络的双重调节。
It has been well documented that that personal significance modulates the high-level cognitiveprocesses, including face recognition, memory and thinking. Whetherpersonal significance (i.e., theself-salience) modulates the visual selection remains poorly understood, however. Here by combiningrecent developed self-associative learning approach and a global-local task, this studypresents the evidencethat self-salience impacted on visual selection by modulatingthe effect of global advantage.Thiseffectconsistently occurred in both divided and focused attention tasks. In contrast, this was not thecase for friend-associations.The present study reported4studies to test wehther and how the self-salienceaffectedthe selection of attention at both behavioral and neural levels.
     Wefirst developed threeexperimentsto measure wether self related information modulated the globaladvantage effect when it was used as target vs. distractorin divided attention task and focused attentiontasks in order to create a behavioral marker on the effect of social salience on attentional selection. In eachexperiment participants carried out two tasks–a self-associative learning task following by a global-localtask. The results in study1showed faster responses to global than local (a global advantage effect)targetswhen both the self-or friend-associated shapes weretargetsand a stranger-associated shapes appreared asdistractors. In contrast, there was no global advantage to stranger-associated shapes in the presence ofself-related distractors while the global advantage effect to stranger-associated shapes survived in thepresence of friend-related distractors. This interference effect of self-salience was verified in the focusedattention task.
     In study2,by comparing divided attention and focused attention as within subject variable, we testedthe relation between self-salience and attention levels (focus vs. divided attention). The results on reactiontimesin study2indicated an enhanced interference effect in the divided than focus attention tasks whenself-associated shapes appeared as distractors.
     The enhanced interference effect was measures in Study3by employing ERPtechnique, in order tomeasure how early the interference effect in visual selection occurs in the brain. The result found that theglobal advangtage of oscillation amplitude took place in the early ERP components P1and N1. Incontrast,The intereference effect of self associated shapeson visual selction occurred in N1. The dataindicated that the modulation of self related in visual search turn out in the early primary visual processstage.
     In order to investagete theneuralbasisunderlying the modulation of self related information inglobal/local process, in Study4we adopted fMRI technique recording the brain activition when participantscarried out a global-local task in an divided attention task. The ROI analyses demonstrated that there wasenhanced activity inbrain regions linked to self-representation [the ventral medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)] in self-associated shapes compared to friend-associated shapes when they appeared as distractors.There was also increased activity in the left intraparietal cortex sulcusin friend-associated shapes but not inself-associated shapes when they displayed as distractors.In contrast, the activity in the right intraparietalcortexwas enhanced when self-associated shapes were targets compared with when friend-associatedshapes ere targets..
     In conclusion, the results demonstrated a reliable rapid learning effect in self-compared with friendassociated shapes. Once participants learnt associations,, the newly learnt self information immediatelymodulatedvisual selction. The effect emerged at the early visual processing (N1) by modulating theinteraction between self-(vmPFC) and attentional (intraparietal cortex)-networks in the brain.
引文
Alexopoulos, T., Muller, D., Ric, F.,&Marendaz, C.(2012). I, me, mine: Automatic attentional capture by self‐related stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology,42(6),770-779.
    Amsterdam, B.(1972). Mirror self‐image reactions before age two. Developmental psychobiology,5(4),297-305.
    Arnell, K. M., Shapiro, K. L.,&Sorensen, R. E.(1999). Reduced repetition blindness for one's own name.Visual Cognition,6(6),609-635.
    Bardi, L., Kanai, R., Mapelli, D.,&Walsh, V.(2013). Direct current stimulation (tDCS) reveals parietalasymmetry in local/global and salience-based selection. Cortex,49(3),850-860.
    Beaucousin, V., Cassotti, M., Simon, G., Pineau, A., Kostova, M., Houdé, O.,&Poirel, N.(2011). ERP evidenceof a meaningfulness impact on visual global/local processing: when meaning captures attention.Neuropsychologia,49(5),1258-1266.
    Berlad, I.,&Pratt, H.(1995). P300in response to the subject's own name. Electroencephalography and ClinicalNeurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section,96(5),472-474.
    Blanca, M., Zalabardo, C., Garcia-Criado, F.,&Siles, R.(1994). Hemispheric differences in global and localprocessing dependent on exposure duration. Neuropsychologia,32(11),1343-1351.
    Brédart, S., Delchambre, M.,&Laureys, S.(2006). Short article one's own face is hard to ignore. The QuarterlyJournal of Experimental Psychology,59(1),46-52.
    Bundesen, C., Kyllingsbaek, S., Houmann, K. J.,&Jensen, R. M.(1997). Is visual attention automaticallyattracted by one’s own name? Perception&Psychophysics,59(5),714-720.
    Cherry, E. C., Halle, M.,&Jakobson, R.(1953). Toward the logical description of languages in their phonemicaspect. Language,34-46.
    Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Dobmeyer, S., Shulman, G. L.,&Petersen, S. E.(1991). Selective and dividedattention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positronemission tomography. The Journal of Neuroscience,11(8),2383-2402.
    Craik, F. I., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M., Stuss, D. T., Winocur, G., Tulving, E.,&Kapur, S.(1999). In searchof the self: A positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science,10(1),26-34.
    Cunningham, S. J., Turk, D. J., Macdonald, L. M.,&Neil Macrae, C.(2008). Yours or mine? Ownership andmemory. Consciousness and Cognition,17(1),312-318.
    Delis, D. C., Robertson, L. C.,&Efron, R.(1986). Hemispheric specialization of memory for visual hierarchicalstimuli. Neuropsychologia,24(2),205-214.
    Devue, C.,&Brédart, S.(2008). Attention to self-referential stimuli: Can I ignore my own face? ActaPsychologica,128(2),290-297.
    Devue, C., Laloyaux, C., Feyers, D., Theeuwes, J.,&Brédart, S.(2009). Do pictures of faces, and which ones,capture attention in the inattentional blindness paradigm? Perception,38(4).
    Devue, C., van der Stigchel, S., Brédart, S.,&Theeuwes, J.(2009). You do not find your own face faster; youjust look at it longer. Cognition,111(1),114-122.
    Dickman, S.(1985). Impulsivity and Perception: Individual Differences in the Processing of the Local andGlobal Dimensions of Stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,48(1),133-149.
    Evans, M., Shedden, J., Hevenor, S.,&Hahn, M.(2000). The effect of variability of unattended information onglobal and local processing: evidence for lateralization at early stages of processing. Neuropsychologia,38(3),225-239.
    Fink, G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J.,&Dolan, R. J.(1996). Where inthe brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees? Nature,382(6592),626-628.
    Fink, G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J.,&Dolan, R. J.(1997). Neuralmechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visualstimuli. Brain,120(10),1779-1791.
    Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W.,&Dolan, R. J.(1998). Hemispheric asymmetries in global/localprocessing are modulated by perceptual salience. Neuropsychologia,37(1),31-40.
    Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J.,&Dolan, R. J.(1997).Hemispheric specialization for global and local processing: the effect of stimulus category. Proceedingsof the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences,264(1381),487-494.
    Gasper, K.,&Clore, G. L.(2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing ofvisual information. Psychological Science,13(1),34-40.
    Geng, H., Zhang, S., Li, Q., Tao, R.,&Xu, S.(2012). Dissociations of subliminal and supraliminal self-facefrom other-face processing: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Neuropsychologia,50(12),2933-2942.
    Gronau, N., Cohen, A.,&Ben-Shakhar, G.(2003). Dissociations of personally significant and task-relevantdistractors inside and outside the focus of attention: a combined behavioral and psychophysiologicalstudy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,132(4),512-529.
    Hübner, R., Volberg, G.,&Studer, T.(2007). Hemispheric differences for global/local processing in dividedattention tasks: Further evidence for the integration theory. Perception,&Psychophysics,69(3),413-421.
    Han, S.,&Jiang, Y.(2006). Neural correlates of within-level and across-level attention to multiple compoundstimuli. Brain research,1076(1),193-197.
    Han, S., Liu, W., Yund, E. W.,&Woods, D. L.(2000). Interactions between spatial attention and global/localfeature selection: an ERP study. Neuroreport,11(12),2753-2758.
    Han, S., Weaver, J. A., Murray, S. O., Kang, X., Yund, E. W.,&Woods, D. L.(2002). Hemispheric asymmetry inglobal/local processing: Effects of stimulus position and spatial frequency. Neuroimage,17(3),1290-1299.
    Harris, C. R.,&Pashler, H.(2004). Attention and the processing of emotional words and names not so specialafter all. Psychological Science,15(3),171-178.
    Harris, C. R., Pashler, H. E.,&Coburn, P.(2004). Moray revisited: High-priority affective stimuli and visualsearch. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A,57(1),1-31.
    Heinze, H. J., Hinrichs, H., Scholz, M., Burchert, W.,&Mangun, G. R.(1998). Neural mechanisms of globaland local processing: A combined PET and ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,10(4),485-498.
    Hillyard, S. A., Vogel, E. K.,&Luck, S. J.(1998). Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism ofselective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philosophical Transactions of theRoyal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences,353(1373),1257-1270.
    Ipata, A. E., Gee, A. L., Gottlieb, J., Bisley, J. W.,&Goldberg, M. E.(2006). LIP responses to a popout stimulusare reduced if it is overtly ignored. Nature neuroscience,9(8),1071-1076.
    Itti, L.(2007). Visual salience. Scholarpedia,2(9),3327.
    Johannes, S., Wieringa, B. M., Matzke, M.,&Münte, T. F.(1996). Hierarchical visual stimuli:electrophysiological evidence for separate left hemispheric global and local processing mechanisms inhumans. Neuroscience letters,210(2),111-114.
    Kaplan, J. T., Aziz-Zadeh, L., Uddin, L. Q.,&Iacoboni, M.(2008). The self across the senses: an fMRI study ofself-face and self-voice recognition. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience,3(3),218-223.
    Kawahara, J.-i.,&Yamada, Y.(2004). Does one's name attract visual attention? Visual Cognition,11(8),997-1017.
    Keenan, J. P., Freund, S., Hamilton, R. H., Ganis, G.,&Pascual-Leone, A.(2000). Hand response differences ina self-face identification task. Neuropsychologia,38(7),1047-1053.
    Keenan, J. P., McCutcheon, B., Freund, S., Gallup, G. G., Sanders, G.,&Pascual-Leone, A.(1999). Left handadvantage in a self-face recognition task. Neuropsychologia,37(12),1421-1425.
    Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S.,&Heatherton, T. F.(2002). Finding the self?An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,14(5),785-794.
    Keyes, H.,&Brady, N.(2010). Self-face recognition is characterized by “bilateral gain” and by faster, moreaccurate performance which persists when faces are inverted. The Quarterly Journal of ExperimentalPsychology,63(5),840-847.
    Kimchi, R.(1988). Selective attention to global and local levels in the comparison of hierarchical. Perception&psychophysics,43(2),189-198.
    Kinchla, R.,&Wolfe, J.(1979). The order of visual processing:“Top-down,”“bottom-up,” or “middle-out”.Perception&psychophysics,25(3),225-231.
    Lamb, M. R.,&Robertson, L. C.(1988). The processing of hierarchical stimuli: Effects of retinal locus,locational uncertainty, and stimulus identity. Perception&psychophysics,44(2),172-181.
    Lamb, M. R.,&Robertson, L. C.(1990). The effect of visual angle on global and local reaction times dependson the set of visual angles presented. Perception&Psychophysics,47(5),489-496.
    Lamb, M. R., Robertson, L. C.,&Knight, R. T.(1989). Attention and interference in the processing of globaland local information: Effects of unilateral temporal-parietal junction lesions. Neuropsychologia,27(4),471-483.
    Lao, J., Vizioli, L.,&Caldara, R.(2013). Culture modulates the temporal dynamics of global/local processing.Culture and Brain,1(2-4),158-174.
    Luck, S. J., Heinze, H., Mangun, G.,&Hillyard, S. A.(1990). Visual event-related potentials index focusedattention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1and N1components.Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology,75(6),528-542.
    Ma, Y.,&Han, S.(2010). Why we respond faster to the self than to others? An implicit positive associationtheory of self-advantage during implicit face recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: HumanPerception and Performance,36(3),619-633.
    Malinowski, P., Hübner, R., Keil, A.,&Gruber, T.(2002). The influence of response competition on cerebralasymmetries for processing hierarchical stimuli revealed by ERP recordings. Experimental brainresearch,144(1),136-139.
    Mangun, G.,&Hillyard, S.(1988). Spatial gradients of visual attention: behavioral and electrophysiologicalevidence. Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology,70(5),417-428.
    Martin, M.(1979). Local and global processing: The role of sparsity. Memory&Cognition,7(6),476-484.
    Martinez, A., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M. I., Frank, L. R., Buxton, R. B., Dubowitz, D.,... Hillyard, S. A.(1999). Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention. Natureneuroscience,2(4),364-369.
    Martinez, A., Moses, P., Frank, L., Buxton, R., Wong, E.,&Stiles, J.(1997). Hemispneric asymmetries in globaland local processing: evidence from fMRI. Neuroreport,8(7),1685-1689.
    Mevorach, C., Hodsoll, J., Allen, H., Shalev, L.,&Humphreys, G.(2010). Ignoring the elephant in the room: aneural circuit to downregulate salience. Journal of Neuroscience,30(17),6072-6079.
    Mevorach, C., Humphreys, G. W.,&Shalev, L.(2006). Opposite biases in salience-based selection for the leftand right posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience,9(6),740-742.
    Mevorach, C., Humphreys, G. W.,&Shalev, L.(2009). Reflexive and preparatory selection and suppression ofsalient information in the right and left posterior parietal cortex. Journal of cognitive neuroscience,21(6),1204-1214.
    Mevorach, C., Shalev, L., Allen, H. A.,&Humphreys, G. W.(2009). The left intraparietal sulcus modulates theselection of low salient stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,21(2),303-315.
    Moray, N.(1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. QuarterlyJournal of Experimental Psychology,11(1),56-60.
    Navon, D.(1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. CognitivePsychology,9(3),353-383.
    Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H.,&Panksepp, J.(2006). Self-referentialprocessing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage,31(1),440-457.
    Pannese, A.,&Hirsch, J.(2010). Self-specific priming effect. Consciousness and Cognition,19(4),962-968.
    Pfister, R., Pohl, C., Kiesel, A., Kunde, W.,&Tsakiris, M.(2012). Your Unconscious Knows Your Name. PLoSONE,7(3), e32402.
    Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P., Gallup Jr, G. G.,&Mohamed, F. B.(2004). Where am I? The neurologicalcorrelates of self and other. Cognitive Brain Research,19(2),114-122.
    Platek, S. M., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., Busch, S., Ruparel, K., Phend, N.,... Langleben, D. D.(2006).Neural substrates for functionally discriminating self‐face from personally familiar faces. Humanbrain mapping,27(2),91-98.
    Poirel, N., Pineau, A.,&Mellet, E.(2006). Implicit identification of irrelevant local objects interacts withglobal/local processing of hierarchical stimuli. Acta Psychologica; Acta Psychologica.
    Poirel, N., Pineau, A.,&Mellet, E.(2008). What does the nature of the stimuli tell us about the GlobalPrecedence Effect? Acta Psychologica; Acta Psychologica.
    Proverbio, A. M., Minniti, A.,&Zani, A.(1998). Electrophysiological evidence of a perceptual precedence ofglobal vs. local visual information. Cognitive Brain Research,6(4),321-334.
    Robertson, L., Lamb, M.,&Knight, R.(1988). Effects of lesions of temporal-parietal junction on perceptual andattentional processing in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience,8(10),3757-3769.
    Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A.,&Kirker, W. S.(1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,35(9),677-688.
    Romei, V., Thut, G., Mok, R. M., Schyns, P. G.,&Driver, J.(2012). Causal implication by rhythmic transcranialmagnetic stimulation of alpha frequency in feature‐based local vs. global attention. European Journalof Neuroscience,35(6),968-974.
    Schatz, J.,&Erlandson, F.(2003). Level-repetition effects in hierarchical stimulus processing: timing andlocation of cortical activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology,47(3),255-269.
    Sergent, J.(1982). The cerebral balance of power: confrontation or cooperation? Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Perception and Performance,8(2),253.
    Shapiro, K. L., Caldwell, J.,&Sorensen, R. E.(1997). Personal names and the attentional blink: a visual"cocktail party" effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,23(2),504.
    Shulman, G. L., Sullivan, M. A., Gish, K.,&Sakoda, W. J.(1986). The role of spatial-frequency channels in theperception of local and global structure. Perception,15(3),259-273.
    Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Jeong, H., Horie, K., Sato, S.,&Kawashima, R.(2008). Face-specific anddomain-general characteristics of cortical responses during self-recognition. Neuroimage,42(1),414-422.
    Sui, J., Chechlacz, M.,&Humphreys, G. W.(2012). Dividing the self: Distinct neural substrates of task-basedand automatic self-prioritization after brain damage. Cognition,122(2),150-162.
    Sui, J., Chechlacz, M., Rotshtein, P.,&Humphreys, G. W.(2013). Lesion-Symptom Mapping ofSelf-Prioritization in Explicit Face Categorization: Distinguishing Hypo-and Hyper-Self-Biases.Cerebral Cortex, bht233.
    Sui, J.,&Han, S.(2007). Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. PsychologicalScience,18(10),861-866.
    Sui, J., He, X.,&Humphreys, G. W.(2012). Perceptual effects of social salience: Evidence fromself-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: HumanPerception and Performance,38(5),1105.
    Sui, J.,&Humphreys, G. W.(2013). Self-referential processing is distinct from semantic elaboration: Evidencefrom long-term memory effects in a patient with amnesia and semantic impairments. Neuropsychologia,51(13),2663-2673.
    Sui, J., Liu, C. H.,&Han, S.(2009). Cultural difference in neural mechanisms of self-recognition. Socialneuroscience,4(5),402-411.
    Sui, J., Liu, C. H., Wang, L.,&Han, S.(2009). Attentional orientation induced by temporarily establishedself-referential cues. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,62(5),844-849.
    Sui, J., Liu, M., Mevorach, C.,&Humphreys, G. W.(2013). The Salient Self: The Left Intraparietal SulcusResponds to Social as Well as Perceptual-Salience After Self-Association. Cerebral Cortex, bht302.
    Sui, J., Rotshtein, P.,&Humphreys, G. W.(2013). Coupling social attention to the self forms a network forpersonal significance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,110(19),7607-7612.
    Sui, J., Sun, Y., Peng, K.,&Humphreys, G. W.(2014). The automatic and the expected self: separating self-andfamiliarity biases effects by manipulating stimulus probability. Attention, Perception,&Psychophysics,1-9.
    Sui, J., Zhu, Y.,&Han, S.(2006). Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: An event-relatedbrain potential study. Neuroreport,17(4),423-427.
    Symons, C. S.,&Johnson, B. T.(1997). The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. PsychologicalBulletin,121(3),371-394.
    Tacikowski, P., Jednoróg, K., Marchewka, A.,&Nowicka, A.(2011). How multiple repetitions influence theprocessing of self-, famous and unknown names and faces: an ERP study. International Journal ofPsychophysiology,79(2),219-230.
    Tong, F.,&Nakayama, K.(1999). Robust representations for faces: evidence from visual search. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,25(4),1016-1035.
    Troje, N. F.,&Kersten, D.(1999). Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces.PERCEPTION-LONDON-,28,483-488.
    Turk, D. J., Brady-van den Bos, M., Collard, P., Gillespie-Smith, K., Conway, M. A.,&Cunningham, S. J.(2013). Divided attention selectively impairs memory for self-relevant information. Memory&Cognition,41(4),503-510.
    Turk, D. J., Cunningham, S. J.,&Macrae, C. N.(2008). Self-memory biases in explicit and incidental encodingof trait adjectives. Consciousness and cognition,17(3),1040-1045.
    Turk, D. J., van Bussel, K., Brebner, J. L., Toma, A. S., Krigolson, O.,&Handy, T. C.(2011). When “it”becomes “mine”: Attentional biases triggered by object ownership. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,23(12),3725-3733.
    Turk, D. J., van Bussel, K., Waiter, G. D.,&Macrae, C. N.(2011). Mine and me: Exploring the neural basis ofobject ownership. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,23(11),3657-3668.
    Vogel, E. K.,&Luck, S. J.(2000). The visual N1component as an index of a discrimination process.Psychophysiology,37(2),190-203.
    Volberg, G.,&Hübner, R.(2004). On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemisphericdifferences in global/local processing: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia,42(13),1805-1813.
    Weissman, D., Giesbrecht, B., Song, A., Mangun, G.,&Woldorff, M.(2003). Conflict monitoring in the humananterior cingulate cortex during selective attention to global and local object features. Neuroimage,19(4),1361-1368.
    Weissman, D.,&Woldorff, M.(2005). Hemispheric asymmetries for different components of global/localattention occur in distinct temporo-parietal loci. Cerebral Cortex,15(6),870-876.
    Wolford, G.,&Morrison, F.(1980). Processing of unattended visual information. Memory&Cognition,8(6),521-527.
    Wood, N.,&Cowan, N.(1995). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: how frequent are attention shifts toone's name in an irrelevant auditory channel? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,and Cognition,21(1),255.
    Yamada, Y., Kawabe, T.,&Miura, K.(2012). One's own name distorts visual space. Neuroscience letters,531(2),96-98.
    Yamaguchi, S., Yamagata, S.,&Kobayashi, S.(2000). Cerebral asymmetry of the top-down allocation ofattention to global and local features. Journal of Neuroscience,20(9), RC72.
    Yovel, G., Yovel, I.,&Levy, J.(2001). Hemispheric asymmetries for global and local visual perception: Effectsof stimulus and task factors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,27(6),1369-1385.
    刘明慧,王凌云,隋洁,&张明.(2012).自我面孔对空间注意的调控:来自Posner空间线索范式的证据.心理科学,35(001),24-29.
    张天阳,&吴艳红.(2013).暂时建立的自我参照线索对返回抑制的影响.心理科学,2,003.
    朱滢,戚健俐,&张剑.(2004).中国学生的自我面孔识别.心理学报,36(4),442-447.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700