用户名: 密码: 验证码:
幼儿掌控动机的发展特点及其与能力、自尊的关系研究
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
掌控动机是人类发展研究领域中的一个核心概念,它是指人们有一种内在动力去理解和影响环境,并在不同程度上与环境发生着有效的相互作用。考虑到掌控动机在个体发展、尤其是早期发展中的重要性,本研究首先考察了掌控动机在幼儿期的表现形式与发展特点;其次,考虑到掌控动机与能力之间存在的相互作用,我们对掌控动机与认知等方面能力之间的关系进行了横向与纵向的探讨;除此,随着儿童社会化程度的加深,社会性成分对掌控动机这种内在驱动力的影响会不断增强。自尊,作为对自我能力和价值的评价性情感体验,可能通过掌控动机对能力的促进或阻碍作用,而受到能力和掌控动机的直接和间接影响。所以,本研究最后考察掌控动机、能力及自尊三者之间可能存在的关系模型。综合所有研究结果如下:
     发展特点:
     (1)对于工具性掌控动机的行为表现,动作掌控主要包括完全由自身发出动作、借助外物发出动作和自我发声(口语运动);物体掌控主要包括使物体运动、使物体发声、以及使物体发生其它变化;社会掌控主要包括直接寻求与成人的接触、间接引起成人的关注、以及自身行为引发他人模仿;
     (2)对于表达性掌控动机的行为表现,儿童在动作和物体掌控行为中获得了较多的积极情感体验,表现在身体动作、面部表情和言语表达中;而在社会掌控中,当掌控活动成功,即教师给予正面反馈,儿童会表现出明显的积极情感反应,即使不成功,情感反应也是不明显的;
     (3)随着儿童年龄增长,动作掌控和物体操作掌控行为事件比例明显减少,社会掌控行为事件比例明显增多;由于身体动作的发展,儿童的动作逐渐由大肌肉到小肌肉过渡,由简单到复杂,表现在动作掌控和物体掌控的行为上;随着儿童言语行为的逐步成熟,语言在社会掌控行为和掌控动机的表达性方面开始发挥作用;在不同年龄段,儿童通过单独的行为、行为语言结合及单独的语言获得社会掌控的事件比例变化明显,反映出幼儿动作与语言发展中的一些关键变化;另外,儿童社会掌控行为的对象逐渐由教师向同伴扩展;
     (4)随年龄增长,幼儿在高认知低效果类材料-拼图上的掌控时间显著提高,掌控变换性显著下降,对低认知高效果类材料-汽车的喜爱度有先下降后上升的趋势;
     (5)4-6岁幼儿在不同材料类型上的平均掌控时间存在显著差异。认知类材料-图书最低,两可类材料-积木最高。对不同材料类型的喜爱度存在显著差异,认知类材料-图书最低,低认知高效果类材料-汽车和两可类材料-积木最高;
     (6)年龄与材料类型对掌控时间和喜爱度的交互作用显著。除了拼图以外,不同年龄(4-6岁)幼儿在其他各类玩具材料上的掌控指向表现出大体的一致性,即随年龄变化比较平缓。但是对高认知低效果类材料-拼图的掌控呈现出显著的上升趋势。另外,不同年龄幼儿对几类材料的喜爱度及其变化趋势具有较大的差异性;
     掌控动机与能力:
     (7)物体掌控动机中的掌控变换性和脱离掌控时间对于两项能力分数均具有显著的负向预测作用;除此,低认知高效果类材料-汽车上的掌控时间显著负向预测了精细动作;认知效果结合类材料-钓鱼上的掌控时间显著负向预测了认知学习;而高认知低效果类材料-拼图上的掌控时间显著正向预测了认知学习;
     (8)控制气质差异后,对于认知学习,认知效果结合类-钓鱼的掌控时间和掌控变换性具有负向的预测作用,而高认知低效果类-拼图的掌控时间具有正向的预测作用。对于精细动作,脱离掌控时间和掌控变换性具有负向的预测作用;
     (9)在低认知高效果任务(探索反馈任务)中,气质的活动性-专注性因素具有显著调节作用。对于活动型幼儿,高水平的探索时间预测了低水平的认知知识,高水平的掌控愉悦感预测了低水平的夹球和低水平的学习方式,但这些关系对于专注型幼儿不显著;
     (10)在高认知低效果任务(困难任务)上成功的幼儿,气质的反应性-抑制性因素在掌控愉悦感对夹球的预测中有显著调节作用。对于抑制型幼儿,来自成功的高掌控愉悦感预测了高水平的夹球技能,而这个关系对于反应型幼儿不显著。在高认知低效果任务上失败的幼儿,气质的活动性-专注性因素在坚持性对学习方式的预测中有显著调节作用。对于专注型幼儿任务中的高坚持性预测了高水平的学习方式,而这个关系对于活动型幼儿不显著;
     (11)对于父母评估的掌控动机,活动性-专注性在物体掌控动机与精细动作(夹球)、物体掌控动机与学习方式以及大动作掌控动机与感觉运动(扔口袋)之间具有显著的调节作用。对于专注型幼儿,高水平的物体掌控动机预测了高水平的精细动作和学习方式,高水平的大动作掌控动机预测了高水平的感觉运动,而这些关系对于活动型幼儿不显著;
     (12)对于父母评估的掌控动机,反应性-抑制性在同伴社会掌控动机和精细动作、感觉运动及学习方式之间分别具有显著的调节作用。对于抑制型幼儿,高水平的同伴社会掌控动机预测了低水平的精细动作、感觉运动和学习方式。反过来,对于反应型幼儿,高水平的同伴社会掌控动机预测了高水平的精细动作;
     掌控动机与能力及自尊:
     (13)在建构的掌控动机、能力、自尊三者的因果模型中,第一个模型从不同材料上的掌控时间到能力,再到自尊中的胜任感,有3条显著路径。在高认知低效果类-拼图上的掌控时间可以解释认知学习能力变异的12%,在低认知高效果类-汽车上的掌控时间可以解释精细动作能力变异的4%,综合能力可以解释自尊中胜任感变异的17%。第二个模型从掌控动机的综合指标(掌控变换性)到认知学习能力,再到自尊中的胜任感,有2条显著路径。掌控变换性可以解释认知学习能力变异的18%,而认知学习能力可以解释自尊中胜任感变异的11%。
Mastery motivation is a core concept in the field of human development, which isdefined that humans are motivated to understand, influence, and interact effectively withenvironment in varying degrees. Given the crucial role of mastery motivation inindividual development, especially in early life, this study firstly investigated themanifestations and characteristics in early childhood. Secondly, concerning the relationsof mastery motivation to other mental functions, we examined the associations of masterymotivation with competence (e.g., cognition and motor skills) in both cross-sectional andlongtudinal researches. In addition, the influence of social factors on this internalmotivation increases along with children’s socialization. Self-esteem, which is defined asthe emotional experience based on self-evaluation of competence and worthiness, may beaffected by competence and mastery motivation directly or indirectly. Therefore, themodels describing the relationships among mastery motivation, competence andself-esteem were built and tested. All the results were concluded as follow:
     Developmental Characteristics:
     (1) With regards to the behavioral mansfestations of instrumental aspects of masterymotivation, motor mastery were performed by children’s own motors without using otherobjects, motors with other objects, or children’s vocalization; object-oriented masterywere performed by making objects move, sound, or change in other ways; social masterywere performed by seeking contact with adults directly, attracting adults’ attentionindirectly, or inspiring other people’s imitation.
     (2) With regards to the behavioral mansfestations of expressive aspects of masterymotivation, children gained much positive emotion experience in motor mastery andobjected-oriented mastery, which was reflected by gestures, expressions and speech;children also gained positive emotion experience when they succeeded in social mastery,and they had no obvious negative emotion even though failing.
     (3) Along with children’s development, the proportion of motor mastery andobject-oriented mastery decreased and that of social mastery increased; children’s motorsin motor mastery and object-oriented mastery transitted from more gross motors to morefine motors and from more simple to more complex; language became to play an important role in social mastery and expressive aspects of mastery motivation; there weresignificant differences among three ages in porportin of behaviors alone, behaviors andlanguage, and language alone to gain social mastery, which reflected the central transitionof motor and language in early development; the objects of children’s social masterywere increasingly expanded from teachers to more people such as other children.
     (4) As children’s ages increased, their persistence on materials with more cognitionengaged and less effect produced improved significantly, and the trasnsition in masterydecreased significantly. Whereas, interest in materials with less cognition engaged andmore effect produced dropped from age4to5and rised from age5-6.
     (5) There were significant differences among persistence on various materials forchildren aged4-6, with lowest persistence on materials with cognition engaged absolutelyand highest persistence on equivocal masterials. In addition, there were significantdifferences among interest in various materials, with lowest interest in materials withcognition engaged absolutely and highest interest in materials with less cognitionengaged and more effect produced and equivocal masterials.
     (6) The interactive influences of age and material type on persistence and interestwere significant. The general trends of persistence for four types of marterials wereparallel from age4to6except materials with more cognition engaged and less effectproduced. In addition, there was a great diversity of interest in various materials atseparate age.
     Mastery Motivation and Competence:
     (7) The transition and off-mastery in object-orientied mastery motivation predictedcognition-learning and fine motor negatively; persistence on materials with less cognitionengaged and more effect produced predicted fine motor negatively; persistence onmaterials with moderate cognition engaged and moderate effect produced predictedcognition-learning negatively; persistence on materials with more cognition engaged andless effect produced predicted cognition-learning positively.
     (8) When temperament was controlled, on cognition-learning, persistence onmaterials with moderate cognition engaged and moderate effect produced and transitionin mastery had negative predictive effects, whereas, persistence on materials with morecognition engaged and less effect produced had a positive predictive effect. On finemotor skills, the transition and off-mastery still had negative predictive effects.
     (9) For the effect-producing task (with less cognition engaged and more effectproduced), the moderation effect of Activity-Attention were significant. For children of Activity Type, higher levels of exploration predicted lower cognition, and higher masterypleasure predicted lower levels of fine motor and appoaches to learning. Theserelationships were not significant for children of Attention Type.
     (10) For the difficult task (with more cognition engaged and less effect produced),the moderation effect of Reactivity-Inhibition and Activity-Attention were significant.For children of Inhibition Type, higher mastery pleasure from success predicted higherlevels of fine motor, whereas the relation was not significant for children of ReactivityType. For children of Attention Type, higher persistence when they failed predictedhigher levels of appoaches to learning, whereas the relation was not significant forchildren of Activity Type.
     (11) For parental report of mastery motivation, the moderation effect ofActivity-Attention was significant. For children of Attention Type, higher object-orientedmastery motivation predicted higher levels of fine motor and appoaches to learning, andhigher gross motor mastery motivation predicted higher levels of sensory motor, whereasthese relations were not significant for children of Activity Type.
     (12) For parental report of mastery motivation, the moderation effect ofReactivity-Inhibition was significant. For children of Inhibition Type, higher socialmastery motivation with children predicted lower levels of fine motor, sensory motor,and appoaches to learning. In turn, for children of Reactivity Type, higher social masterymotivation with children predicted higher levels of fine motor.
     Mastery Motivation, Competence and Self-esteem
     (13) According to persistence on various materials and general mastery (off masteryand transition in mastery), two models were built for describing the relationships amongmastery motivation, competence and self-esteem. In the first model, there were threesignificant paths. Persistence on materials with more cognition engaged and less effectproduced explained12%of variance in cognition-learning, and Persistence on materialswith less cognition engaged and more effect produced explained4%of variance in finemotor. Further, the latent variable of competence explained17%of variance in perceivedcompetence of self-esteem. In the second model, there were two significant paths. Thetransition in mastery explained18%of variance in cognition-learning andcognition-learning explained11%of variance in perceived competence of self-esteem.
引文
[1]彭聃龄.普通心理学[M].北京:北京师范大学出版社,2001.319.
    [2] Pintrich P R, Schunk D H. Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications [M].Eaglewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall,1996.
    [3] MacTurk R H, Morgan G A. Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualizations, and applications[M]. Norwood, NJ: Ablex,1995.
    [4] White R W. Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence [J]. Psychological Review,1959,66:297-333.
    [5] Hunt J McV. Intelligence and experience [M]. New York: Ronald Press,1961.
    [6] Friedlander B Z, Sterritt G M, Kirk G E. Exceptional infant: Assessment and intervention [M].New York: Bruner/Mazel,1975.491-502.
    [7] Bjorklund D F, Green B L. The adaptive nature of cognitive immaturity [J]. American Psychologist,1992,47:46-53.
    [8] Messer D J. Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and socialprocesses [M]. London: Routledge,1993.36-54.
    [9] Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Vol.2, Separation [M]. New York: Basic Books,1973.203.
    [10] Gilmore L, Cuskelly M. A longitudinal study of motivation and competence in children withDown syndrome: Early childhood to early adolescence [J]. Journal of Intellectual DisabilityResearch,2009,53:484-492.
    [11] Shonkoff J P, Phillips D A. From neurons to neighbourhoods: The science of early childhooddevelopment [M]. Washington DC: National Academy Press,2000.
    [12] Turner L A, Johnson B. A model of mastery motivation for at-risk preschoolers [J]. Journal ofEducation Psychology,2003,95:495-505.
    [13] Harter S, Zigler E. The assessment of effectance motivation in normal and retarded children [J].Developmental Psychology,1974,10:169-180.
    [14] Yarrow L J, Rubenstein J L, Pedersen F A. Infant and environment: Early cognitive andmotivational development [M]. Washington, DC: Wiley,1975.
    [15] Harter S. Developmental differences in the manifestation of mastery motivation onproblem-solving tasks [J]. Child Development,1975,46:370-378.
    [16] Harter S. Effectance motivation reconsidered [J]. Human Development,1978,21:34-64.
    [17] Harter S. The effects of social reinforcement and task difficulty level on the pleasure derivedby normal and retarded children from cognitive challenge and mastery [J]. Journal ofExperimental Child Psychology,1977,24:476-494.
    [18] Harmon R J, Morgan G A, Glicken A D. Continuities and discontinuities in affective andcognitive-motivational development [J]. International Journal of Child Abuse and Neglect,1984,8:157-167.
    [19] MacTurk R, Hunter F, McCarthy M, et al. Social mastery motivation in Down syndrome andnondelayed infants [J]. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education,1985,4:93-109.
    [20] Yarrow L J, Morgan G A, Jennings K D, et al. Infants’ persistence at tasks: relationships tocognitive functioning and early experience [J]. Infant Behavior and Development,1982,5:131-142.
    [21] Yarrow L J, McQuiston S, MacTurk R H, et al. Assessment of mastery motivation during thefirst year of life: contemporaneous and cross-age relationships [J]. Developmental Psychology,1983,19,159-171.
    [22] Atkinson J W. Motivational determinants of risk taking behavior [J]. Psychological Review,1957,64:359-372.
    [23] Feather N T. The study of persistence [J]. Psychological Bulletin,1962,59:94-115.
    [24] Collins W A. Minnesota symposium on child psychology [M]. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum,1980.137-202.
    [25] Morgan G A, Busch-Rossnagel N A, Maslin-Cole C A, et al. Mastery motivation tasks: Manualfor15-to36-month-old children [M]. New York: Fordham University,1992.
    [26] Castaldi J, Hrncir E J, Caldwell C B. Future models for the study of individual differences inmotivation during infancy [J]. Early Education and Development,1990,1:385-393.
    [27] Schaefer C E, Gitlin K, Sandgrund A. Play diagnosis and assessment [M]. New York: Wiley,1991.11-38.
    [28] Wenar C. Executive competence in toddlers [J]. Genetic Psychology Monographs,1976,93:189-285.
    [29] Belsky J, Garduque L, Hrncir E. Assessing performance, competence, and excutive capacity ininfant play: Relations to home environment and security of attachment [J]. DevelopmentalPsychology,1984,20:406-417.
    [30] Belsky J, Most R K. From exploration to play: A cross sectional study of infant free-playbehavior [J]. Developmental Psychology,1981,17:630-639.
    [31] McGrath M, Sullivan M C, Brem F, et al. Mastery motivation and cognitive development in4year old children born at various degrees of medical risk [J]. Journal of Pediatric Nursing,1995,10:287–295.
    [32] Busch-Rossnagel N A. Mastery motivation in toddlers [J]. Infants and Young Children,1997,9:1–11.
    [33] Cain K M, Dweck C S. The relation between motivational patterns and achievement cognitionsthrough the elementary school years [J]. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,1995,41:25–52.
    [34] Weiner I B, Craighead W E. The corsini encyclopedia of psychology [M]. Hoboken, NJ, USA:John Wiley&Sons,2010.868-870.
    [35] Morgan G A, Harmon R J, Maslin-Cole C A. Mastery motivation: definition and measurement[J]. Early Education and Development,1990,1:318–339.
    [36] Messer D J. Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement, and socialprocesses [M]. London: Routledge,1993.
    [37] Emde R N, Harmon R J. Continuities and discontinuities in development [M]. New York:Plenum,1984.263-291.
    [38] Lewis M. Origins of intelligence (2nd ed.)[M]. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum,1983.451-477.
    [39] Messer D J. Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and socialprocesses [M]. London: Routledge,1993.168-185.
    [40] Maslin C A, Morgan G A. Measures of social competence: Toddlers social and objectorientation during mastery tasks [C]. Presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society forResearch in Child Development, Toronto, Canada.1985.
    [41] Vondra J I, Jennings K D. Infant mastery motivation: The issue of discriminant validity [J].Early Education and Development,1990,1:340-353.
    [42] Wachs T D. Specificity of environmental actions as manifest in environmental correlates ofinfant’s mastery motivation [J]. Developmental Psychology,1987,23:782-790.
    [43] Schaefer C E, Gitlin K, Sandgrund A. Play diagnosis and assessment [M]. New York: JohnWiley&Sons,1991.65-86.
    [44] Morgan G A, Harmon R J, Maslin-Cole C A, et al. Parent and teacher perceptions of youngchildren’s mastery motivation: Assessment and review of research. In D. J. Messer (Ed.),Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes [M].London: Routledge,1993.109-131.
    [45] Harter S. A model of mastery motivation in children: Individual differences and developmentalchange [C]. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), The Minnesota symposium on child psychology: Vol.14.Aspects of the development of competence. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum,1981.215-255.
    [46] Jennings K D, Yarrow L J, Martin P P. Mastery motivation and cognitive development: alongitudinal study from infancy to3.5years of age [J]. International Journal of BehavioralDevelopment,1984,7:441-461.
    [47] Molnar J M, Weisz J R. The Pursuit of Mastery by Preschool Boys and Girls: An ObservationalStudy [J]. Child Development,1981,52(2):724-727.
    [48] Harter S. Pleasure derived by children from cognitive challenge and mastery [J]. ChildDevelopment,1974,45(3):661-669.
    [49] David M B, Jonathan R. Pleasure associated with cognitive mastery as related to children'sconceptual tempo [J]. Child Development,1976,47(3):881-884.
    [50] Barrett K C, Morgan G A, Maslin-Cole C. Three studies on the development of masterymotivaiton in infancy and toddlerhood. In D. J. Messer (Ed.), Mastery motivation in earlychildhood: Development, measurement and social processes [M]. London: Routledge,1993.83-108.
    [51] Morgan G A, Maslin-Cole C A, Biringen Z, et al. Play assessment of mastery motivation ininfants and young children. In C. E. Schaefer, K. Gitlin and A. Sandgrund (Eds.), Playdiagnosis and assessment [M]. New York: John Wiley&Sons,1991.65-86.
    [52] Kelley S A, Brownell C A, Campbell S B. Mastery motivation and self-evaluative affect intoddlers: Longitudinal relations with maternal behavior [J]. Child Development,2000,71(4):1061-1071.
    [53] Boggiano A K, Main D S, Katz P. Mastery motivation in boys and girls: the role of intrinsicversus extrinsic motivation [J]. Sex Roles,1991,25(9/10):511-520.
    [54] Jennings K D. Early development of mastery motivation and its relation to the self-concept [C].In M. Bullock (Ed.), The development of intentional action: Cognitive, motivational andinteractive processes. Contributions to human development (Vol.22). Basel, Switzerland:Karger,1991.1-13.
    [55] Piaget J. The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, Trans.)[M]. New York: InternationalUniversities Press,1952(Original work published1936).
    [56] Frantz R L, Fagan J F, Miranda S B, et al. Early visual selection [C]. In Cohen L B, Salapatek P.(Eds.) Infant Perception: From Sensation to Cognition (Vol.1). New York: Academic Press,1975.
    [57] Sullivan M W, Lewis M. Emotion and cognition in infants: facial expressions duringcontingency learning [J]. International Journal of Behavioral Development.1989,12:221-237.
    [58] Watson J S, Ramey C T. Reactions to response contingent stimulation early in infancy [J].Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,1972,18:219-227.
    [59] Messer D J, McCarthy M E, McQuiston S, et al. Relations between mastery motivation ininfancy and competence in early childhood [J]. Developmenta1Psychology,1986,22:336-372.
    [60] Kagan J. The second year: The emergence of self-awareness [M]. Cambridge: MA, HarvardUniversity Press,1981.
    [61] Bullock M, Lutkenhaus P. The development of volitional behavior in the toddler years [J].Child Development.1988,59:664-674.
    [62] Stipek D, Recchia S, McClintic S. Self-evaluation in young children [J]. Monographs of thesociety for research in child development.1992,57:1, Serial No.226.
    [63] Heckhausen H. Emotional components of action: their ontogeny as reflected in achievementbehavior. In Gorlitz, D., Wohlwill, J. F.(Eds.) Curiosity, Imagination, and Play: On theDevelopment of Spontaneous Cognitive and Motivational Processes [M]. Hillsdale: NJ,Erlbaum,1987.
    [64] Lewis M, Sullivan S W, Stanger C, et al. Self development and self-conscious emotions [J].Child Development,1989,60:146-156.
    [65] Geppert U, Kuster U. The emergence of “wanting to do it oneself”: a precursor of achievementmotivation [J]. International Journal of Behavioral Development,1983,6:355-369.
    [66] Schneider K. The development of emotions in preschool children during achievement-orientedstriving [C]. Paper presented at the China Satellite International Society for the Study ofBehavioral Development Conference, Beijing, China,1987.
    [67] Schneider K, Hanne K, Lehmann B. The development of children’s achievement-relatedexpectancies and subjective uncertainty [J]. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,1989,47:160-174.
    [68] Redding R E, Morgan G A, Harmon R J. Mastery motivation in infants and toddlers: is itgreatest when tasks are moderately challenging?[J]. Infant Behavior and Development,1988,11:419-430.
    [69] Heckhausen H. Developmental precursors of success and failure experience. In d’Ydewalle, G.,Lens, W.(Eds.) Cognition in Human Motivation and Learning, motivation [M]. Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum,1981.
    [70] Bird J E, Thompson G B. Understanding of the dimensional terms “easy” and “hard” in theself-evaluation of competence [J]. International Journal of Behavioral Development,1986,9:343-357.
    [71] Heckhausen H. The development of achievement motivation [C]. In Hartup, W. W.(Ed.)Review of Child Development Research (Vol6). Chicago: University of Chicago Press,1982.
    [72] Lewis M, Alessandri S M, Sullivan M W. Individual differences in shame and pride as afunction of children’s gender, task difficulty, and parental attribution [C]. Presented at thebiennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA,1991.
    [73] Jennings K D, Connors R E, Stegman C E. Does a physical handicap alter the development ofmastery motivation during the preschool years?[J]. Journal of the American Academy of Childand Adolescent Psychiatry,1988,27:312-317.
    [74] MacTurk R H, Hunter F, McCarthy M E, et al. Social mastery motivation in Down syndromeand nondelayed infants [J]. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education,1985,4:93-109.
    [75] Belsky J, Garduque L, Hrncir E. Assessing performance, competence, and excutive capacity ininfant play: Relations to home environment and security of attachment [J]. DevelopmentalPsychology,1984,20:406-417.
    [76] Hrncir E J, Speller G M, West M. What are we testing?[J]. Developmental Psychology,1985,21,226-232.
    [77] Jennings K D, Harmon R J, Morgan G A, et al. Exploratory play as an index of masterymotivation: Relationships to persistence, cognitive functioning and environmental measures [J].Developmental Psychology,1979,15:386-394.
    [78] Morgan G A, Harmon R J, Maslin-Cole C A, et al. Assessing perceptions of mastery motivation:The Dimensions of Mastery Questionnaire, its development, psychometrics and use [G]. Ft.Collins: Colorado State University, Human Development and Family Studies Department,1992
    [79] Yarrow L J, Pedersen F A. The interplay between cognition and motivation in infancy. In M.Lewis (Ed.), Origins of intelligence: Infancy and early childhood [M]. New York: Plenum,1976.379-399
    [80] Yarrow L J, Klein R, Lomonaco S, et al. Cognitive and motivational development in earlychildhood. In B. Z. Friedlander, G. M. Sterritt, and G. E. Kirk,(Eds.), Exceptional infant:Assessment and intervention [M]. New York: Bruner/Mazel,1975.491-502.
    [81] Atkinson J W. Motivational determinants of risk taking behavior [J]. Psychological Review,1957,64:359-372.
    [82] Hunt J. Intrinsic motivation and its role in psychological development [C]. In D. Levine (Ed.),Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol.13). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press,1965.189-282.
    [83] Tolman E C. Purposive behavior in animals and man [M]. New York: Appleton-Century,1932.
    [84] Weiner B, Kun A, Benesh-Weiner M. The development of mastery, emotions, and morality froman attributional perspective [C]. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposium on childpsychology (Vol.14). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum,1980.327-402
    [85] Gaiter J L, Morgan G A, Jennings K D, et al. Variety of cognitively-oriented caregiver activities:Relationships to cognitive and motivational functioning at1and3.5years of age [J]. Journal ofGenetic Psychology,1982,141:49-56.
    [86] Morgan G A, Harmon R J. Developmental transformations in mastery motivation: Measurementand validation. In R. N. Emde and R. J. Harmon (Eds.), Continuities and discontinuities indevelopment [M]. New York: Plenum,1984.263-291.
    [87] Makin N D. The role of hearing in language development. In J. F. Kavanaugh(Ed.), Otitis mediaand child development [M]. Parkton, MD: York,1986.3-11.
    [88] Berger J, Cunningham C C. Development of eye contact between mothers and Normal versusDown syndrome infants [J]. Developmental Psychology,1981,17:678-689.
    [89] Vietze P, McCarthy M, McQuiston S, et al. Attention and exploratory behavior in infants withDown syndrome. In T. Field and A. Sostek (Eds.), Infants born at risk: Perceptual andphysiological processes [M]. New York: Grune and Stratton,1983.251-268.
    [90] Lewis M, Brooks-Gunn J. Age and handicapped group difference in infants’ visual attention [J].Child Development,1984,55:858-868.
    [91] Miranda S B, Fantz R L. Recognition memory in Down’s syndrome and normal infants [J]. ChildDevelopment,1974,45:651-660.
    [92] McCarthy M E, McQuiston S. The relationship of contingent parental behaviors to infantmotivation and competence [C]. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society forResearch in Child Development, Detroit, MI,1983.
    [93] Yarrow L J, MacTurk R H, Vietze P M, et al. Developmental course of parental stimulation andits relationship to mastery motivation during infancy [J]. Developmental Psychology,1984,20:492-503.
    [94] MacTurk R H, McCarthy M E, Vietze P M, et al. Sequential analysis of mastery behavior in6-and12-month-old infants [J]. Developmental Psychology,1987,23(2):199-203.
    [95] Morgan G A, Clup R E, Busch-Rossnagel N A, et al. A longitudinal study of mastery motivationin infants9to25months of age (abstract)[C]. In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska symposium onmotivation (Vol.40): Developmental perspectives on motivation. Lincoln: University ofNebraska Press,1993.273.
    [96] Fung A Y. The relationship of mother’s perception to the child’s competence and masterymotivation [Z]. Unpublished master’s thesis. University of Manitoba, Winnipeg,1984.
    [97] Morgan G A, Harmon R J, Pipp S, et al. Assessing mother’s perceptions of mastery motivation:The utility of the MOMM questionnaire [Z]. Unpublished manuscript,1983.
    [98] Jennings K D, Connors R E, Stegman C E, et al. Mastery motivation in young preschoolers:Effect of a physical handicap and implications for educational programming [J]. Journal of theDivision for Early Childhood,1985,9:162-169.
    [99] Banta J T. Tests for the evaluation of early childhood education: The Cincinnati autonomy testbattery [C]. In J. Hellmuth (Ed.) Cognitive Studies (Vol.1). New York: Bruner/Mazel,1970.206-278.
    [100] Block J H, Block J. The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior[C]. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), The Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol.13). Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum,1980.294-356.
    [101] Similansky S. The effects of sociodramatic play on disadvantaged preschool children [M].New York: Wiley,1968.
    [102] Butterfield P M, Miller L. Read your baby: A follow up intervention program for parents withNICU infants [J]. Infant Mental Health,1984,5(2):107-116.
    [103] Brockman L M. Mastery motivation and competence in young children [C]. Paper presentedat the NIH workshop on Mastery Motivation, Bethesda, MD,1984.
    [104] Maslin C A, Bretherton I, Morgan G A. Toddler mastery motivation and competence: Linkswith attachment security, maternal scaffolding, and family climate. In D. J. Messer (Ed.),Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes [M].London: Routledge,1993.205-229.
    [105] Sarimski K, Warndorf P K. Zur Beurteilung der Ansdauer bei zielgerichteten Tatigkeiten inder Psychodiagnostik geistigbehinderter Kinder (Assessment of persistence in goal-directedactivities and psycho-diagnostics of mentally handicapped children)[J]. Sonderpadagogik,1991,21:210-216.
    [106] Morgan G A, Busch-Rossnagel N A, Barrett K C, et al. The Dimensions of MasteryQuestionnaire (DMQ): A manual about its development, psychometrics and use [Z].2009.Retrieved from http://www.mycahs.colostate.edu, on June10th2010.
    [107] Morgan G. A, Bartholomew S. Assessing mastery motivation in7-and10-year olds [C]. In G. A.Morgan&N. A. Busch-Rossnagel (Chairs), New measures of mastery motivation for infancythrough elementary school. Symposium conducted at the Fourth Head Start National ResearchConference, Washington D C,1998.
    [108] Hebb D O. The organization of behavior [M]. New York: Wiley,1949.
    [109] Bakeman R, Brown J V. Behavioral dialogues: An approach to the assessment of mother-infantinteraction [J]. Child Development,1977,48:195-203.
    [110] Sackett G. P. Measurement in observational research [C]. In G. P. Sackett (Ed.), Observingbehavior (Vol.2): Data collection and analysis methods. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press,1978.25-44.
    [111] MacTurk R H, Vietze P M, McCarthy M E, et al. The organization of exploration behavior inDown syndrome and nondelayed infants [J]. Child Development,1985,56:573-581.
    [112] Feinman S. Social referencing in infancy [J]. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,1982,28:445-470.
    [113] Morgan G A, Busch-Rossnagel N A, Maslin-Cole C A. Mastery motivation tasks: Manual for15-to36-month-old children [G]. New York: Fordham University, Psychology Department,1992.
    [114] Morgan G A, Maslin-Cole C A, Ridgeway D, et al. Toddler mastery motivation and aspects ofmother-child affect communication [J]. Program and Proceeding of the DevelopmentalPsychobiology Research Group Retreat,1988,5:15-16.
    [115] Niccols A, Atkinson L, Pepler D. Mastery motivation in young children with Down syndrome:Relations with cognitive and adaptive competence [J]. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,2003,47:121–133.
    [116] Scarr S. Testing for children: assessment of the many determinants of intellectual competence[J]. American Psychologist,1981,36:1159–1166.
    [117] Ulvand S E. Cognition and motivation in early infancy: an interactionist approach [J]. HumanDevelopment,1980,23:17–32.
    [118] Deci E L, Ryan R M. Intrinsic motivation [M]. In I. B. Weiner,&W. E. Craighead,(Eds.), Thecorsini encyclopedia of psychology. Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley&Sons,2010.868-870.
    [119] Hauser-Cram P, Krauss M W, Warfield M E, et al. Congruence and predictive power of mothers’and teachers’ ratings of mastery motivation in children with mental retardation [J]. MentalRetardation,1997,35:355–363.
    [120] Barrett K C, Morgan G A. Continuities and discontinuities in mastery motivation during infancyand toddlerhood: A conceptualization and review [M]. In R. H. MacTurk&G. A. Morgan (Eds.),Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualizations, and applications. Norwood, NJ: AblexPublishing Corporation,1995.57-93.
    [121] Sigman M, Cohen S E, Beckwith L, et al. Task persistence in2-year-old preterm infants inrelation to subsequent attentiveness and intelligence [J]. Infant Behavior and Development,1987,10(3):295-305.
    [122] Schwethelm B, Mahoney G. Task persistence among organically impaired mentally retardedchildren [J]. American Journal of Mental Deficiency,1986,90:432-439.
    [123] Hauser-Cram P. Mastery motivation in three-year old children with Down syndrome [M]. In D. J.Messer (Ed.), Mastery Motivation in Early Childhood: Development, Measurement SocialProcesses. London: Routledge,1993.230-249.
    [124] Yarrow L J, Pederson F. The interplay between cognition and motivation in infancy [M]. In M.Lewis (Ed.), Origins of Intelligence. New York, NY: Plenum Press,1976.379-399.
    [125] Vondra J I. Parental support for exploratory motivation and competence across the first3yearsof life [J]. Dissertation Abstracts International,1987,47:2758.
    [126] Lynch M M, Hayden L C, Seifer R, et al. Mastery motivation, child competence and familycontext [J]. Infant behavior and development,1996,19(1):593.
    [127] Lange G., MacKinnon C E, Nida R E. Knowledge, strategy, and motivational contributions topreschool children’s object recall [J]. Developmental Psychology,1989,25:772-779.
    [128] Banerjee P, Tamis-Lemonda C. Infants’ persistence and mothers’ teaching as predictors oftoddlers’ cognitive development [J]. Infant Behavior and Development,2007,30:479-491.
    [129] Gottfried A E, Gottfried A W. Toward the development of a conceptualization of giftedmotivation [J]. Gifted Child Quarterly,2004,48:121-132.
    [130] Brockman L M, Morgan G A, Harmon R J. Mastery motivation and developmental delay [M].In T. Wachs&R. Sheehan (Eds.), Assessment of young developmentally disabled children.New York: Plenum,1988.267-283.
    [131] McGrath M, Sullivan M C, Brem F, Rocherolle K C. Mastery motivation and cognitivedevelopment in4-year-old children born at various degrees of medical risk [J]. Journal ofPediatric Nursing,1995,10(5):287-295.
    [132] Gilmore L, Cuskelly M. A longitudinal study of motivation and competence in children withDown syndrome: Early childhood to early adolescence [J]. Journal of Intellectual DisabilityResearch,2009,53:484-492.
    [133] Turner L A, Johnson B. A model of mastery motivation for at-risk preschoolers [J]. Journal ofEducation Psychology,2003,95:495-505.
    [134] Dweck C S. Motivational processes affecting learning [J]. American Psychologist,1986,41:1040–1048.
    [135] Elliot E S, Dweck C S. Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement [J]. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology,1988,54:5-12.
    [136] King M, Turner L A. The relation of parenting practices to perceptions of control [C]. Paperpresented at the conference of the Southeastern Psychological Association, Atlanta, GA.2001.
    [137] Skinner E A. Perceived control, motivation, and coping [M]. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage,1995.
    [138] Skinner E A, Wellborn J G, Connell J P. What it takes to do well in school and whether I’vegot it: A process model of perceived control and children’s engagement and achievement inschool [J]. Journal of Educational Psychology,1990,82:22-32.
    [139] Turner L A. Relation of attributional beliefs to memory strategy use in children andadolescents with mental retardation [J]. American Journal of Mental Retardation,1998,103:162–172.
    [140] Cool V A, Keith T Z. Testing a model of school learning: direct and indirect effects onacademic achievement [J]. Contemporary Educational Psychology,1991,16:28-44.
    [141] Skinner E A, Belmont M J. Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacherbehavior and student engagement across the school year [J]. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology,1993,85:571–581.
    [142] Broussard S C, Garrison M E B. The relationship between classroom motivation andacademic achievement in elementary-school-aged children [J]. Family and Consumer SciencesResearch Journal,2004,33:106-120.
    [143] Schiefele U, Krapp A, Winteler A. Interest as a predictor of academic achievement: Ameta-analysis of research [M]. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi,&A. Krapp (Eds.), The role ofinterest in learning and development, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum,1992.183–212.
    [144] Cheung C R, McBride-Chang C. Relation of Perceived Maternal Parenting Style, Practice,and Learning Motivation to Academic Competence in Chinese Children [J]. Merril-PalmerQuarterly,2008,54(1):1-22.
    [145] Yarrow L J, Morgan G A, Jennings K D, et al. Mastery motivation: a concept in need ofmeasures [C]. Symposium presented at the Biennial Southeastern Conference on HumanDevelopment, Nashville, Tenn.1976.
    [146] Gilmore L, Cuskelly M, Purdie N. Mastery motivation: stability and predictive validity fromages two to eight [J]. Early Education&Development,2003,14:411–424.
    [147] Morrow J D, Camp B W. Mastery motivation and temperament of7-month-old infants [J].Pediatric Nursing,1996,22:211-217.
    [148]田录梅,李双.自尊概念辨析[J].心理学探新,2005,25(2):26-29.
    [149]刘双,张向葵.婴幼儿自尊的前兆与形成[J].学前教育研究,2008,(10):26-30.
    [150] James W. The principles of psychology [M]. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press(Original work published1890),1983.296-297.
    [151] Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image [M]. Princeton, NJ: Princeton UniversityPress,1965.
    [152] Branden N. The psychology of self-esteem [M]. New York: Bantam,1969.110.
    [153] Mruk C. Self-esteem: research, theory, and practice [M]. New York: Springer,1995.102.
    [154]张向葵,刘双.西方自尊两因素理论研究回顾及其展望[J].心理科学,2008,31(2):494-499.
    [155] Brown J D. The self [M]. New York: McGraw-Hill,1998.173-176.
    [156] Harter S. Development perspectives on the self-system [C]. Handbook of Child Psychology:Social and personality development (vol.4). New York: Wiley,1983.275-385.
    [157] Harter S. The development of self-representation [C]. In W. Damon (Series Ed.)&N. Eisenberg(Vol. Ed.). Handbook of child psychology: Vol.3. Social, emotional, and personalitydevelopment (5th ed). New York: Wiley,1998.553-617.
    [158]左志宏,席居哲.母婴关系与自我发展——Mahler的分化/个体化理论评介[J].心理科学,2003,26(1):182-183.
    [159]庞丽娟,李辉.婴儿心理学[M].杭州:浙江教育出版社,1993.361-389.
    [160] Lewis M, Brooks-Gunn. Social cognition and the acquisition of self [M]. New York: Plenum,1979.
    [161] Stern D. The international world of the infant [M]. New York: Basic Books,1985.
    [162] Bandura A. Competence considered [M]. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press,1990.316-352.
    [163] Case R. Stages in the development of the young child’s first sense of self. DevelopmentalReview [J].1991,11:210-230.
    [164] Sander L. Explorations in child psychiatry [C]. In E.J. Anthony(Ed.). New York: Plenum Press,1975.129-166.
    [165] Santrock J W. Children (8th ed.)[M]. New York: McGraw-Hill,2005.249.
    [166] Goldsmith H H, Buss A H, Plomin R, et al. What is temperament? Four approaches [J]. ChildDevelopment,1987,58:505-529.
    [167] Hubert N C, Wachs T D, Peters-Martin P, et al. The study of early temperament: Measurementand conceptual issues [J]. Child Development,1982,53:571-600.
    [168] Rothbart M K, Hwang J. Temperament and the development of competence and motivation[M]. In A. J. Elliot&C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence&motivation. New York:Guilford Press,2005.167-184.
    [169]刘文,杨丽珠.基于教师评定的3-9岁儿童气质结构[J].心理学报,2005,1:67-72.
    [170] Eisenberg N, Fabes R A. Emotion, regulation, and the development of social competence [C].In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Vol.14. Emotion andsocial behavior. Newbury Park, CA: Sage,1992.119-150.
    [171] Eisenberg N, Fabes R A, Bernzweig J, et al. The relations of emotionality and regulation topreschoolers' social skills and sociometric status [J]. Child Development,1993,64:1418-1438.
    [172] Larsen R J, Diener E. Affect intensity as an individual difference characteristic: A review [J].Journal of Researchin Personality,1987,2(1):1-39.
    [173] Eisenberg N, Fabes R A, Murphy B, et al. The role of emotionality and regulation inchildren's social functioning: A longitudinal study [J]. Child Development,1995,66:1360-1384.
    [174] Murphy B C, Eisenberg N, Fabes R A, et al. Consistency and change in children’s emotionalityand regulation: A longitudinal study [J]. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,1999,45:413–444.
    [175] Rubin K R, Coplan R J, Fox N A, et al. Emotionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers'social adaptation [J]. Development and Psychopathology,1995,7:49-62.
    [176] Chen X, Chen H, Li D, et al. Early Childhood Behavioral Inhibition and Social and SchoolAdjustment in Chinese Children: A5-year Longitudinal Study [J]. Child development,2009,80:1692-1704.
    [177] Chen X, Hastings P, Rubin K H, et al. Child rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition inChinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study [J]. Developmental Psychology,1998,34:677–686.
    [178] Pérez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, McDermott J M, et al. Attention biases to threat and behavioralinhibition in early childhood shape adolescent social withdrawal [J]. Emotion,2010,10:349-357.
    [179] Williams L R, Degnan K A, Perez-Edgar K E, et al. Impact of behavioral inhibition andparenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood throughadolescence [J]. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,2009,37:1063-1075.
    [180] Gaertner B M, Spinrad T L, Eisenberg N. Focused Attention in Toddlers: Measurement,Stability, and Relations to Negative Emotion and Parenting [J]. Infant and Child Development,2008,17(4):339–363.
    [181] Ruff H A, Rothbart M K. Attention in early development: Themes and variations [M]. New York:Oxford University Press,1996.
    [182] Chen C. The impact of temperament, personality, and perceived parenting on Chineseadolescents' achievement motivation and academic achievement [D]. Hong Kong: TheUniversity of Hong Kong,2010.
    [183] Guerin D W, Gottfried A W, Oliver P H, et al. Temperament: Infancy through adolescence:The Fullerton longitudinal study [M]. New York: Plenum Press,2003.
    [184] Oliver P H, Guerin D W, Gottfried A W. Temperamental task orientation: Relation to high schooland college educational accomplishments [J]. Learning and Individual Differences,2007,17(3):220-230.
    [185] Chess S. Temperament and learning ability of school children [J]. American Journal of PublicHealth,1968,58(12):2231-2239.
    [186] Martin R. Activity level, distractibility and persistence: critical characteristics in early schooling[M]. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates,&M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood.Chichester: Wiley,1989.283-298.
    [187] Martin R P, Drew K D, Gaddis L R, et al. Prediction of elementary school achievement frompreschool temperament: Three studies [J]. School Psychology Review,1988,17:125-137.
    [188] Martin R P, Holbrook J. Relationship of temperament characteristics to the academicachievement of first grade children [J]. Journal of Psychological Assessment,1985,3:131-140.
    [189] Martin R P, Nagle R, Paget K. Relationships between temperament and classroom behavior,teacher attitudes, and academic achievement [J]. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,1983,1:377–386.
    [190] Coplan R J, Barber A M, Lagacé-Séguin D G. The role of child temperament as a predictor ofearly literacy and numeracy skills in preschoolers [J]. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,1999,14:537-553.
    [191] Bono M A, Stifter C A. Maternal attention-directing strategies and infant focused attentionduring problem solving [J]. Infancy,2003,4:235–250.
    [192] Lawson K R, Ruff H A. Early attention and negative emotionality predict later cognitive andbehavioral function [J]. International Journal of Behavioral Development,2004,28:157-165.
    [193] Fox H A, Henderson H A, Marshall P J, et al. Behavioral inhibition: Linking biology andbehavior within a developmental framework [J]. Annual Review of Psychology,2005,56:235–262.
    [194] Kagan J. Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity [J]. Child Development,1997,68:139-143.
    [195] Kellam S G, Ensminger M E, Simon M B. Mental health in first grade and teenage alcohol andcigarette use [J]. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,1980,5:273–304.
    [196] Kerr M, Tremblay R E, Pagani L, et al. Boys’ behavioral inhibition and the risk of laterdelinquency [J]. Archives of General Psychiatry,1997,54:809–816.
    [197] Asendorpf J B. Development of inhibited children’s coping with unfamiliarity [J]. ChildDevelopment,1991,62:1460-1474.
    [198] Asendorpf J B, Denissen J J A, van Aken M A G. Inhibited and aggressive preschool children at23years of age: Personality and social transition into adulthood [J]. Developmental Psychology,2008,44:997-1011.
    [199] Gest S D. Behavioral inhibition: Stability and association with adaptation from childhood toearly adulthood [J]. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,1997,72:467-475.
    [200] Kagan J, Reznick J S, Snidman N, et al. Childhood derivatives of inhibition and lack ofinhibition to the unfamiliar [J]. Child Development,1988,59:1580-1589.
    [201] Else-Quest N, Hyde J, Goldsmith H, et al. Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis[J]. Psychological Bulletin,2006,132:33-72.
    [202] Buss A H, Plomin R. A temperament theory of personality development [M]. New York: Wiley,1975.
    [203] Eaton W O, Enns L R. Sex differences in motor activity level [J]. Psychological Bulletin,1986,100:19–28.
    [204] Hegvik R L, McDevitt S C, Carey W B. The Middle Childhood Temperament Questionnaire [J].Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,1982,3:197-200.
    [205] Rowe D C, Plomin R. Temperament in early childhood [J]. Journal of Personality Assessment,1977,41:150–156.
    [206] Schoen M J, Nagle R J. Prediction of school readiness from kindergarten temperament scores [J].Journal of School Psychology,1994,32(2):135-147.
    [207] Dettling A, Gunnar M, Donzella B. Cortisol levels of young children in full-day childcarecenters: Relations with age and temperament [J]. Psychoneuroendocrinology,1999,24:519-536.
    [208] Susman E, Schmeelk K, Ponirakis A, et al. Maternal prenatal, postpartum, and concurrentstressors and temperament in3year-olds: A person and variable analysis [J]. Development andPsychopathology,2001,13:629-652.
    [209] Strelau J. A regulative theory of temperament [J]. Australian Journal of Psychology,1983,35:305-317.
    [210] Goldsmith H H, Campos J J. Fundamental issues in the study of early temperament: the DenverTwin Temperament Study [M]. In M. E. Lamb&A. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmentalpsychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum,1986.231-283.
    [211] Eisenberg N, Guthrie I K, Fabes R A, et al. The relations of regulation and emotionality toresilience and competent social function in elementary school children [J]. Child Development,1997,68:295-311.
    [212] Barkley R A. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing aunifying theory of ADHD [J]. Psychological Bulletin,1997,121:65–94.
    [213] Kochanska G, Murray K T, Harlan E T. Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity andchange, antecedents, and implications for social development [J]. Developmental Psychology,2000,36:220–232.
    [214] Rothbart M K, Ahadi S A. Temperament and the development of personality [J]. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology,1994,103:55-66.
    [215] Eisenberg N, Fabes R A, Guthrie I K, et al. Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their rolein predicting quality of social functioning [J]. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,2000,78:136–157.
    [216] Gray J A. The psychology of fear and stress [M]. London: Weidenfeld&Nicholson,1971.
    [217] Schwebel D C, Plumert J. Longitudinal and concurrent relations among temperament, abilityestimation, and injury proneness [J]. Child Development,1999,70:700-712.
    [218]张向葵,刘秀丽.发展心理学[M].长春:东北师范大学出版社,2002.
    [219] Harter S. The development of self-esteem [M]. In Kernis. M. H.(Ed.), Self-esteem issues andanswers. Psychology Press: New York and Hove,2006.144-145.
    [220]张丽华.3-9岁儿童自尊结构、发展特点及其相关影响因素的研究[D].大连:辽宁师范大学,2004.
    [221]董奇,申继亮.心理与教育研究法[M].杭州:浙江教育出版社,2005.370-372.
    [222] Stern D. The international world of the infant [M]. New York: Basic Books,1985.36.
    [223] Bandura A. Competence considered [M]. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press,1990.316-352.
    [224]秦金亮.儿童发展概论[M].北京:高等教育出版社,2008.180-183.
    [225] Lewis M, Brooks-Gunn. Social cognition and the acquisition of self [M]. New York: Plenum,1979.52-63.
    [226]孟昭兰.婴儿心理学[M].北京:北京大学出版社,1997.391,140-144.
    [227]但菲,刘彦华.婴幼儿心理发展与教育[M].北京:人民出版社,2008.418-421.
    [228]张向葵,桑标.发展心理学[M].北京:教育科学出版社,2012.155-160.
    [229]赵玲.培养幼儿观察能力的几点探讨[J].学前教育研究,1998,01:47-48.
    [230]邱学青.学前儿童游戏(第四版)[M].南京:江苏教育出版社,2008.
    [231]盖笑松.儿童入学准备研究与实践[M].长春:吉林教育出版社,2007.
    [232]刘文,杨丽珠,邹萍.3-9岁儿童气质类型研究[J].心理与行为研究,2004,04:603-609.
    [233]刘文.3-9岁儿童气质发展及其与个性相关因素关系的研究[D].大连:辽宁师范大学,2002.
    [234] Palisin H. Preschool temperament and performance on achievement tests [J]. DevelopmentalPsychology,1986,22(6):766-770.
    [235] Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.)[M]. Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum,1988.
    [236] Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, et al. Statistical power analyses using G*Power3.1: Tests forcorrelation and regression analyses [J]. Behavior Research Methods,2009,41:11491160.
    [237] Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A G, et al. G*Power3: A flexible statistical power analysis programfor the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences [J]. Behavior Research Methods,2007,39:175-191.
    [238] Pamela E D, Howard M S. A Meta-Analysis of Measures of Self-esteem for Young Children: AFramework for Future Measures [J]. Child Development,2001,72:887-906.
    [239] Sommer B, Sommer R. A practical guide to behavioral research (4th ed.)[M]. New York:Oxford University Press,1997.
    [240]张丽华,杨丽珠.三种情境下4~8岁儿童自尊发展的实验研究[J].心理科学,2006,29(2):327-331.
    [241] Schneider K. Achievement-related emotions in preschoolers [M]. In F. Halisch&J. Kuhl (Eds.).Motivation, intention, and volition. New York: Springer,1987.163-177.
    [242] Matheny A P. Temperament and cognition: relations between temperament and mental scores
    [M]. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates,&M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood.Chichester: Wiley,1989.263–282.
    [243] Newman J, Noel A, Chen R, et al. Temperament, selected moderating variables and earlyreading achievement [J]. Journal of School Psychology,1998,36(2):215-232.
    [244] Wills T A, Cleary S D, Filer M, et al. Temperament related to early-onset substance use: Test ofa developmental model [J]. Prevention Science,2001,2:145-163.
    [245] Dixon Jr W E, Salley B J, Clements A D. Temperament, Distraction, and Learning inToddlerhood [J]. Infant Behavior and Development,2006,29(3):342-357.
    [246] Coplan R J, Girardi A, Findlay L C, et al. Understanding solitude: Young children’s attitudes andresponses toward hypothetical socially withdrawn peers [J]. Social Development,2007,16:390-409.
    [247] Coplan R J, Rubin K H, Fox N A, et al. Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone:Distinguishing among reticence and passive and active solitude in young children [J]. ChildDevelopment,1994,65:129-137.
    [248] Fox N A, Schmidt L A, Calkins S D, et al. The role of frontal activation in the regulation anddysregulation of social behavior during the preschool years [J]. Development andPsychopathology,1996,8:89-102.
    [249] Thomas A, Chess S. Temperament and development [M]. New York: Brunner/Mazel,1977.
    [250] Wachs T D, Gruen G E. Early experience and human development [M]. New York: Plenum,1982.
    [251]杨丽珠,张丽华.3-9岁儿童自尊结构研究[J].心理科学,2005,28(1):23-27.
    [252] Payne G,耿培新,梁国立.人类动作发展概论[M].北京:人民教育出版社,2008.
    [253] Ruff H A, Lawson K R. Development of sustained, focused attention in young children duringfree play [J]. Developmental Psychology,1990,26:85–93.
    [254]彭聃龄.普通心理学[M].北京:北京师范大学出版社,2001.
    [255] Kostelnik等著,邹晓燕等译.儿童社会性发展指南[M].北京:人民教育出版社,2009
    [256] Isabella R, Belsky J. International synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachment: areplication study [J]. Child Development,1991,62:373-384.
    [257] Rofe Y. Stress and affiliation: A utility theory. Psychological Review,1984,91:251-268.
    [258] Lamb M E, Campos J. Development in Infancy [M]. New York: Random House,1982.
    [259]李幼穗.儿童社会性发展及其培养[M].上海:华东师范大学出版,2004.
    [260] Zhou Q, Hofer C, Eisenberg N, et al. The developmental trajectories of attentional focusing,attentional and behavioral persistence, and externalizing problems during school-age years [J].Developmental Psychology,2007,43:369-385.
    [261] Stipek D J, Ryan R H. Economically disadvantaged preschoolers: Ready to learn but further togo [J]. Developmental Psychology,1997,33:711–723.
    [262] George J, Greenfield D B. Examination of a structured problem-solving flexibility task forassessing approaches to learning in young children: Relation to teacher ratings and children’sachievement [J]. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,2005,26:69–84.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700