用户名: 密码: 验证码:
空间定向中返回抑制的脑机制研究
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
视觉空间注意定向是大多数生物及人类最基本的功能之一,它在快速的目标搜索中具有重要意义。空间定向包括两种机制:内源和外源定向。内源定向是指有目的地分配注意资源到预先计划的空间位置,而外源定向则是对突出性事件的反射性自动反应。对于外源定向,其线索化位置最初的易化效应被归功于外周线索吸引注意的结果,而后对线索化位置的反应减慢被称为返回抑制(inhibition of return, IOR)。
     IOR通常采用经典的空间线索化范式进行研究。在这类实验中,当线索靶刺激间隔时间(stimulus onset asynchrony, SOA)小于250ms时被试对线索化位置的靶刺激反应快于非线索化位置,而当SOA大于250ms时,则表现为对线索化位置的一个慢反应。它表征了对线索化位置及其附近的突出刺激的反应执行减弱,以此来抑制注意重新定向到线索化位置。该效应最初被认为是阻碍注意返回先前搜索过的位置。自Posner和Cohen报道该效应以来,IOR已成为空间定向研究中的一个重要成分,并成为一个颇有争议的研究热点。
     各种不同的技术应用于IOR相关的研究,主要包括行为学研究、事件相关电位(event-related potentials, ERPs)研究、功能磁共振成像(functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI)研究、经颅磁刺激(transcranial magnetic stimulation, TMS)研究、单细胞记录(signal-cell recording)、正电子断层扫描成像(positron emission computerized topography, PET)等。
     本论文围绕视觉空间定向中返回抑制的脑机制问题,利用高时间分辨率的事件相关电位(ERPs),及其低分辨率层析成像(low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography, LORETA)源定位技术,和高空间分辨率的功能磁共振技术等,对该问题进行了深入、细致、有创新性的研究。所做的主要工作与成果如下:
     1、视觉空间线索化效应的动态脑机制研究
     在cue-target范式中,研究了由外周非信息化线索所引起的脑电,并将其结果分阶段进行了LORETA定位。与以往采用ERP研究IOR的方式不同的是,在本研究中我们关注的是由靶刺激出现前的线索所激活的脑区的时间特性。这些结果显示:在线索出现后,相关的激活大致可以分为三个阶段:早阶段(110ms-240ms)的激活为前额叶皮层,顶内皮层,以及对侧枕颞皮层。中阶段(240ms-350ms)的激活主要分布在额叶皮层和顶叶皮层。晚阶段(350ms-650ms)的主要激活在枕顶皮层,但与第一阶段不同的是激活区域转移到线索化位置的同侧。这些发现表明,IOR与注意和动作反应都有关,最初的易化和IOR在时程上是共存的,并受两种神经网络调制。最后我们提出了一个计时器模型将IOR的空间机制延伸到一个时空机制。
     2、基于按键反应抑制的返回抑制脑机制研究
     采用非信息外周线索化Go/Nogo任务实验,通过ERPs的早成分(P1和N1)和晚成分(Go/Nogo N2和P3)来刻画IOR的神经机制。头表拓扑地形图和LORETA显示,线索化效应调制的早成分主要分布在背侧枕顶区域;晚成分主要分布在额叶中部区域。Nogo-N2在有效试验中比在无效试验中出现得早且其幅度也相对要小,这种现象表明,IOR相关的晚成分被反应准备抑制所调制。Nogo-P3在有效中比在无效中出现得迟且其幅度相对要大,该结果表明额叶眼区(FEF,frontal eye field)在线索化位置有一个解标记的过程。这些证据表明,IOR的机制既包括感觉抑制也包括反应抑制。
     3、空间定向和按键抑制交互效应的脑机制研究
     以前的研究报道前额叶皮层(PFC, prefrontal cortex),包括背侧PFC(DLPFC)、额下回(IFC,inferior frontal cortex )、额中回内侧(MFC,medial frontal cortex)和前扣带回(ACC,the anterior cingulate cortex)在按键反应抑制中起重要作用。然而,对于相关脑区是如何调制空间定向和控制冲突的交互效应却仍然不清楚。因此,本研究中采用事件相关fMRI技术来研究空间定向和反应抑制的交互效应所涉及的不同神经机制。两类图形(按键刺激和抑制按键刺激)随机呈现在线索化或非线索化位置。内源反应抑制激活大范围皮层,包括两侧颞顶连接区域(TPJ, temporoparietal junctions)、两侧额上回(superior frontal gyrus)、右侧颞上回(superior temporal gyrus)、以及额下回(inferior frontal gyrus)。与之相反,外源反应抑制仅仅激活右侧额上回和右侧额中回。同时在本研究中也涉及到空间定向的时程效应,内源定向的时程效应主要体现在两侧MiFG (middle frontal gyrus)和MeFG(medial frontal gyrus)的激活,而外源定向的时程效应主要体现在两侧的MiFG激活。这些结果表明在不同的定向情况下,反应抑制所涉及到的脑区不同,则它们可能反应的是分离的加工过程。
     4、视觉搜索任务中的返回抑制脑机制研究
     外源定向被认为是反射性的自动加工过程。它最初表现为易化效应然后被一个延时的反应(IOR)所替代。然而,对于外源易化和IOR是一个加工过程的两个阶段还是两个相互独立的加工过程仍然不清楚。一些研究采用保持内源注意的方法来研究IOR,发现易化和IOR是相互独立的过程。到目前为止,很少有报道探讨外源注意易化和IOR的相互关系。在本研究中,我们利用高时间分辨率的ERP技术在视觉搜索任务中对外源易化和IOR的关系进行了研究。当外源注意保持在序列搜索位置上时仍然获得了一个延时(IOR),伴随此行为的有三个ERP成分:后顶的Pd200、前额叶的Nd240、和两侧TPJ的Nd280。当外源注意保持在平行搜索后的位置时,获得了一个快反应(外源易化效应)。同时发现了一个ERP成分:枕顶中部的Nd280。这些结果表明外源易化和IOR涉及不同的脑区和/或神经过程,它们可能反映的是独立的和分离的加工过程。
The orienting of visual-spatial attention, which is fundamental to most organisms, plays an important role in rapid and efficient search of visual environments. This process is thought to include two mechanisms: endogenous and exogenous orienting. Endogenous orienting refers to the purposeful allocation of attentional resources to a predetermined location in space, whereas exogenous orienting is thought to be triggered reflexively and automatically by an abrupt onset event. The early benefit of exogenous orienting at the cued location is usually attributed to the capture of attention by the peripheral cue, and the subsequent decline in performance at the cued location has been referred to as inhibition of return (IOR).
     IOR is studied by using the typical spatial cue-target paradigm. In such an experiment, subjects respond faster to a target presented at a cued location than that at an uncued location when a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between cue and target is shorter than 250 ms, whereas subjects respond slower to a target appearing at a cued location than that at an uncued location when SOA is longer than 250 ms. IOR is thought to represent a decline in salience in the vicinity of the cue that discourages re-orienting back to the cued location. The primary assumed mechanism is that attention is inhibited from returning to previously searched locations. Since Posner & Cohen’s seminal study, IOR has become an actively investigated component of orienting, and has been a hot topic of many debates.
     Various technologies are employed to the studies related to IOR. These include behavioral studies, event-related potentials (ERPs), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), signal-cell recording, positron emission computerized topography (PET) and so on.
     This dissertation focuses on the studies of brain mechanisms of IOR by high temporal resolution ERPs with the source localization of low resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) and high spatial resolution fMRI respectively. A series of multilevel, novel and detailed researches on IOR have been carried out. Our major works and achievements are listed as below:
     1、A study on the dynamic brain mechanism of visual spatial-cueing effect
     Within the cue-target paradigm, this study analyses the ERPs elicited by the peripheral uninformative cue and uses LORETA to localize the results of the different processing stages. Unlike previous ERP investigations of IOR, in this study we focus on the time characteristic of the neural activity (via EEG) elicited by the cue prior to the appearance of the target. The results show that after cue onset, activations may be approximately divided into three stages. In the early stage (110ms-240ms), activations are in the prefrontal cortex, the intraparietal cortex, and the contralateral occipito-temporal cortex. In the middle stage (240ms-350ms), activations are mainly in the frontal cortex and the parietal cortex. In the late stage (350ms-650ms), the main activations are in the occipito-parietal cortex. Unlike the early stage, the activation areas shift to the ipsilateral side of the cued location. These findings indicate that IOR is related to both attention and motor response, and also suggest that the time course of initial facilitation and IOR is concurrent and mediated by two neural networks. Finally, a time table is proposed, which extends the spatial mechanism of IOR to a spatio-temporal mechanism.
     2、A study on the brain mechanism of IOR based on the response inhibition
     With an uninformative peripheral cued Go/Nogo task experiment, this study aims to characterize the neural mechanism of IOR by both early (P1 and N1) and late (Go/Nogo N2 and P3) ERPs. The scalp topographies and LORETA show that the changes of the early ERPs, the cueing effects, are distributed mainly over the dorsal occipito-parietal areas. The changes of the late ERPs are distributed mainly over the frontocentral areas. The Nogo-N2 is earlier and smaller in valid trials than in invalid trials, which suggests that the late components related to IOR are modulated by response preparation inhibition. The Nogo-P3 is later and larger in valid trials than that in invalid trials, indicating that the frontal eye field(FEF)is free from an inhibitory marker in the cued locations. These evidences suggest that the mechanism of IOR consists of both the sensory inhibition and the response preparation inhibition.
     3、A study on the brain mechanism of interaction between spatial orienting and response inhibition
     The previous studies have suggested that the prefrontal cortex (PFC), including dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), inferior frontal cortex (IFC), medial frontal cortex (MFC) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), plays an important role in response inhibition in Go/Nogo tasks. However, it remains unclear how these“executive”brain regions will act when the conflict control process interacts with spatial orienting. Therefore, event-related fMRI is used to investigate the differential neural mechanisms underlying interactions between spatial orienting and response inhibition. Two types of figures (go stimulus and Nogo stimulus) are random presented either at the cued location or uncued location. Endogenous response inhibition activates widespread cortical regions including bilateral temporoparietal junctions, bilateral superior frontal gyrus, right superior temporal gyrus, and inferior frontal gyrus. Conversely, exogenous response inhibition activates only two areas including right superior frontal gyrus and right middle frontal gyrus. Meanwhile, the interaction of the timecourse in spatial orienting has been investigated in this study. The timecourse of endogenous orienting indicates activations in the medial frontal gyrus, whereas the timecourse of exogenous orienting indicates activations in the bilateral middle frontal gyrus. These results show that response inhibition in different orienting involves different brain areas, thus they are quite possibly two dissociable processes.
     4、A study on the brain mechanism of IOR in visual search tasks
     Exogenous orienting is considered to be a reflexive and automatic process. It initiates facilitation, which is then replaced by a delayed response (i.e. IOR). However, it is still unclear whether the initial facilitation and the later IOR are two stages of one process or simply two independent processes. Some studies have observed IOR when endogenous attention is fixed and found that these processes are driven by separate mechanisms. To date, however, few studies have directly addressed the relationship between exogenous attention facilitation and IOR. Here, we investigate the different effects of exogenous facilitation and IOR in visual search tasks by recording ERPs with high temporal resolution. When exogenous attention remains to be fixed at the serial search locations, a delayed response is observed (i.e. IOR) with three ERP components: a Pd200 at the posterior parietal areas, an Nd240 at the middle tended to left prefrontal areas and an Nd280 at the bilateral temporoparietal areas. When exogenous attention stays at parallel search locations, a faster response is observed (exogenous facilitation). Here, one ERP component appears, an Nd280 at the middle occipito-parietal areas. These results suggest that the exogenous facilitation and IOR involve different brain areas and/or neural processes, and are therefore most likely two dissociable processes.
引文
[1] Cheal ML and Lyon DR. Central and peripheral precuing of forcedchoice discrimination, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.A, Human Experimental Psychology,1991,43: 859-880
    [2] Jonides J. Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye movement, In: Long, J.B., Baddeley, A.D. (Eds.), Attent. Perform., vol. IX. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1981, 187-203
    [3] Müller HJ and Rabbitt PMA. Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption, J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 1989, 15: 315-330
    [4] Posner MI, Nissen MJ and Ogden WC. Attended and unattended processing models: the role of set for spatial locations, In: Pick, H.L., Saltzman, F.J. (Eds.), Modes of Perceiving and Processing Information. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, 1978, 137-157
    [5] Posner MI and Cohen Y. Components of visual orienting, In: Bouma, H., Bouwhis, D. (Eds.), Attent. Perform., vol. X. Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, 1984, 531-556
    [6] Maylor EA. Facilitatory and inhibitory components of orienting in visual space, In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI: Mechanisms of intention. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum., 1985, 189-204.
    [7] Tipper SP, Rafal RD, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Starrveldt Y, Ro T, Egly R, Danziger S and Weaver B. Object based facilitation and inhibition from visual orienting in the human split, J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perf 1997, 23: 1522-1532
    [8] Klein RM. Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search, Nature, 1988, 334: 430-431
    [9] Posner MI. Orienting of attention, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology, 1980, 32: 3-25
    [10] Klein RM. On the role of endogenous orienting in the inhibitory aftermath of exogenous orienting, In U. Mayr, E. Awh and S. Keele Developing Individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael Posner. Washington, DC: APA Books, 2005, 45-64
    [11] Posner MI, Rafal RD, Choate LS and Vaughan J. Inhibition of return: Neural basis and function, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1985, 2: 211-228
    [12] Klein RM. Inhibition of return, Trends Cogn. Sci., 2000, 4: 138-147
    [13] Tassinari G, Aglioti S, Chelazzi L, Peru A and Berlucchi G. Do peripheral non-informative cues induce early facilitation of target detection? Vision Research, 1994, 34:179-189
    [14] Pratt J and Abrams RA. Inhibition of return in discrimination tasks, J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perf., 1999, 25:229-242
    [15] Pratt J, Kingstone A and Khoe W. Inhibition of return in location- and identity-based choice decisions, Perception & Psychophysics, 1997, 59: 964-971
    [16] Klein RM and Dick B. Temporal dynamics of reflexive attention shifts: A dual-stream rapid serial visual presentation exploration, Psychological Science, 2002, 13: 176-179
    [17] Treisman A. Features and objects in visual processing, Scientific American, 1986, 254: 114-125
    [18] Yantis S and Jonides J. Attentional capture by abrupt onset: New perceptural objects or visual masking, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 1996, 22: 1505-1513
    [19] Treisman A and Gelade G. A feature omtegration theory of attention, Cognitive Psychology, 1980, 12: 97 - 136
    [20] Klein RM and Taylor TL. Categories of cognitive inhibition with reference to attention, In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Car (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. San Diego: Academic Press, 1994
    [21] Wolfe JM and Pokorny CW. Inhibitory tagging in visual search: A failure to replicate, Percept Psychophys, 1990, 48: 357-362
    [22] Müller HJ and von Mühlenen A. Probing distractor inhibition in visual search: Inhibition of return, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 2000, 26: 1591-1605
    [23] Takeda Y and Yagi A. Inhibitory tagging in visual search can be found if search stimuli remain visible, Perception & Psychophysics, 2000, 62: 927-934
    [24] Duncan J and Humphreys GW. Visual search and stimulus similarity, Psychological review, 1989, 96: 433-485
    [25] Wolfe JM. Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1994, 1: 202-238
    [26] Reuter-Lorenz PA, Jha AP and Rosenquist JN. What is inhibited in inhibition of return? J. Exp. Psycho.: Hum. Percept. Perf., 1996, 22: 367-378
    [27] Taylor TL and Klein RM. On the causes and effects of inhibition of return, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1998, 5
    [28] Taylor TL and Klein RM. Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2000, 26: 1639-1656
    [29] Godijn R and Theeuwes J. Oculomotor capture and inhibition of return: evidence for an oculomotor suppression account of IOR, Psychol. Res, 2002, 66: 234-246
    [30] Godijn R and Theeuwes J. The Relationship Between Inhibition of Return and Saccade Trajectory Deviations, J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 2004, 30: 538-554
    [31] Tassinari G, Aglioti S, Chelazzi L, Marzi CA and Berlucchi G. Distribution in the visual field of the costs of voluntarily associated attention and of the inhibitory after-effects of covert orienting, Neuropsychologia, 1987, 25: 55-71
    [32]罗跃嘉主编.认知神经科学教程,北京:北京大学出版社, 2006
    [33]尧德中.脑功能探测的电学理论与方法,北京:科学出版社, 2003
    [34] Prime DJ and Ward LM. Inhibition of return from stimulus to response, Psychological Science, 2004, 15: 272-276
    [35] Prime DJ and Ward LM. Cortical expressions of inhibition of return, Brain Research, 2006, 1072: 161-174
    [36] McDonald JJ, Ward LM and Kiehl KA. An event-related brain potential study of inhibition of return, Percept. Psychophys, 1999, 61: 1411-1423
    [37] Hopfinger JB and Mangun GR. Tracking the influence of reflexive attention on sensory and cognitive processing, Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2001, 1: 56-65
    [38] Wascher E and Tipper SP. Revealing effects of noninformative spatial cues: an EEG study of inhibition of return, Psychophysiology, 2004, 41: 716-728
    [39] Eimer M. An ERP study on visual spatial priming with peripheral onsets, Psychophysiology, 1994, 31: 154-163
    [40] Hopfinger JB and Mangun GR. Reflexive attention modulates processing of visual stimuli in human extrastriate cortex, Psychological Science, 1998, 9: 441-447
    [41] Chica AB and Lupiá?ez J. Effects of endogenous and exogenous attention on visual processing: An inhibition of return study, Brain Res, 2009, 75-85
    [42] Doallo S, Lorenzo-Lopez L, Vizoso C, Holguin SR, Amenedo E and Bara S. The time course of the effects of central and peripheral cues on visual processing: an event-related potentials study, Clin Neurophysiol, 2004, 115: 199-210
    [43] Tian Y and Yao D. A Study on the Neural Mechanism of Inhibition of Return by the Event-Related Potential in the Go/Nogo Task, Biological Psychology, 2008, 79: 171-178
    [44] Van der Lubbe RHJ, Vogel RO and Postma A. Different Effects of Exogenous Cues in a Visual Detection and Discrimination Task: Delayed Attention Withdrawal and/or Speeded Motor Inhibition? Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 2005, 17: 1829-1840
    [45]唐孝威主编.脑功能成像,合肥:中国科技大学出版社, 1999
    [46] Zhou X and Chen Q. Neural correlates of spatial and non-spatial inhibition of return(IOR) in attentional orienting, Neurophchologia, 2008, 46: 2766-2775
    [47] Mayer AR, Dorflinger JM, Rao SM and Seidenberg M. Neural networks underlying endogenous and exogenous visual-spatial orienting, Neuroimage, 2004, 23: 534-541
    [48] Mayer AR, Seidenberg M, Dorflinger JM and Rao SM. An event-related fMRI study of exogenous orienting: supporting evidence for the cortical basis of inhibition of return? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2005, 16: 1262-1271
    [49] Chen Q, Wei P and Zhou X. Distinct neural correlates for resolving stroop conflict at inhibition of return in human auditory system, Exp. Brain Res., 2006, 176: 630-640
    [50] Lepsien J and Pollmann S. Covert reorienting and inhibition of return: An event-related fMRI study, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 2002, 14: 127-144
    [51] Ro T, Pratt J and Rafal RD. Inhibition of return in saccadic eye movements, Exp. Brain Res., 2000, 130: 264-268
    [52] Dorris MC, Klein RM, Everling S and Munoz DP. Contribution of the primate superior colliculus to inhibition of return, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2002, 14: 1256-1263
    [53] Dorris MC, Taylor TL, Klein RM and Munoz DP. Neural correlate of inhibition of return (IOR): Visual and motor preparatory signals in the monkey superior colliculus (SC), Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, 1998
    [54] Lupiá?ez J, Klein RM and Bartolomeo P. Inhibition of return: twenty years after, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2006
    [55] Samuel AG and Kat D. Inhibition of return: A graphical meta-analysis of its time course, and an empirical test of its temporal and spatial properties, Psychonom. Bull. Rev., 2003, 10: 897-906
    [56] Lupiá?ez J, Milán EG, Tornay FJ, Madrid E and Tudela R. Does IOR occur in discrimination tasks? Yes, it does, but later, Perception & Psychophysics, 1997, 59: 1241-1254
    [57] Larrison-Faucher A, Briand KA and Sereno AB. Delayed onset of inhibition of return in schizophrenia. Progr. Neuro-psychopharmacol, Biol. Psychiatry, 2002, 26: 505-512
    [58] MacPherson AC, Klein RM and Moore CM. Inhibition of return in children and adolescents, J. Exp. Child Psychol, 2003, 85: 337-351
    [59] Tucker DM. Spatial sampling of head electrical fields: the geodesic sensor net, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1993, 87: 154-163
    [60] Fuchs M and Drenckhahn R. An improved boundary element method for realistic volume-conductor modeling, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 1998, 45: 980-997
    [61] Xu P, Tian Y, Chen H and Yao D. Lp norm iterative sparse solution for EEG source localization, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 2007, 54: 400-409
    [62] Fletcher EM, Kussmaul CL and Mangun GR. Estimation of interpolation errors in scalp topographic mapping, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1996, 98: 422-434
    [63] Yao D. High-resolution EEG mappings: a spherical harmonic spectra theory and simulation results, Clinical Neurophysiology, 2000, 111: 81-92
    [64] Pascual-Marqui RD, Michel CM and Lehmann D. Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain, International Journal of Psychophysiology, 1994, 18: 49-65
    [65] Xu P, Tian Y, Lei X, Hu X and Yao D. Equivalent Charge Source Model Based Iterative Maximum Neighbor Weight for Sparse EEG Source Localization, Annals of Biomedical Engineering, 2008, 36: 2051-2067
    [66] Talairach J and Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain, Thieme, New York, 1988
    [67] Yao D, Wang L, Oostenveld R, Nielsen KD, Arendt-Nielsen L and Chen A. A comparative study of different references for EEG spectral mapping: the issue of the neutral reference and the use of the infinity reference, Physiol. Meas., 2005, 26: 173-184
    [68] Di Russo F, Martinez A and Hillyard SA. Source Analysis of Event-related Cortical Activity during Visuo-spatial Attention, Cerebral Cortex, 2003, 13: 486-499
    [69] Di Russo F, Martinez A, Sereno MI, Pitzalis S and Hillyard SA. Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential, Human Brain Mapping, 2001, 15: 95-111
    [70] Danziger S, Fenrich R and Rafal RD. Inhibitory tagging of locations in the blind field of hemianopic patients, Consciousness & Cognition, 1997, 6: 291-307
    [71] Hopfinger J. Electrophysiology of reflexive attention, In L. Itti, G. Rees, & J. Tsotsos (Eds.), Encyclopedia on the neurobiology of attention 2005, 219-235
    [72] Kingstone A and Pratt J. Inhibition of return is composed of attentional and oculomotor process, Percept. Psychophys, 1999, 61: 1046-1054
    [73] Handy TC, Jha AP and Mangun GR. Promoting novelty in vision: Inhibition of return modulates perceptual-level processing, Psychological Science, 1999, 10: 157-161
    [74] Schmidt WC. Inhibition of return is not detected using illusory line motion, Percept. Psychophys., 1996, 58: 883-898
    [75] Ivanoff J and Klein RM. The presence of a nonresponding effector increases inhibition of return, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2001, 8: 307-314
    [76] Geczy I, Czigler I and Balazs L. Effects of cue information on response production and inhibition measured by event-related potentials, Acta Physiol Hung, 1999, 86: 37-44
    [77] Eimer M. Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/Nogo task, Biol Psychol, 1993, 35: 123-138
    [78] Kopp B, Mattler U, Goertz R and Rist F. N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming, Electroenceph clin Neurophysiol, 1996, 99: 19-27
    [79] Thorpe S, Fize D and Marlot C. Speed of processing in the human visual system, Nature, 1996, 381:520-522
    [80] Falkenstein M, Hoormann J and Hohnsbein J. ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition, Acta Psychol (Amst), 1999, 101: 267-291
    [81] Tekok-Kilic A, Shucard JL and Shucard DW. Stimulus modality and go/nogo effects on P3 during parallel visual and auditory continuous performance tasks, Psychophysiology 2001, 38: 578-589
    [82] Rafal R, Davies J and Lauder J. Inhibitory tagging at subsequently fixated locations: Generation of "inhibition of return" without saccade inhibition, Visual Cognition, 2006, 13: 308-323
    [83] Simson R, Ritter W and Vaughan HGJ. Effects of expectation on negative potentials during visual processing, Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 1985, 62: 25-31
    [84] Fecteau JH and Munoz DP. Correlates of Capture of Attention and Inhibition of Return across Stages of Visual Processing, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2005, 17: 1714-1727
    [85] Aron AR, Robbins TW and Podrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex, Trends Cogn. Sci., 2004, 8: 170-177
    [86] Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA and Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control, Science, 2004, 306: 443-447
    [87] Ikeda A, Luders HO, Collura TF, Burgess RC, Morris HH, Hamano T and Shibasaki. Subdural potentials at orbitofrontal and mesial prefrontal areas accompanying anticipation and decisionmaking in humans: a comparison with Bereitschaftspotential, Electroenceph clin Neurophysiol, 1996, 98: 206-212
    [88] Sasaki K, Gemba H, Nambu A and Matsuzaki R. No-go activity in the frontal association cortex of human subjects, Neurosci. Res., 1993, 18: 249-252
    [89] Casey BJ, Trainor RJ, Orendi JL, Schubert AB, Nystrom LE, Giedd JN, Castellanos X, Haxby JV, Noll DC, Cohen JD, Forman SD, Dahl RE and Rapoport JL. A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a go-no go task, J Cogn Neurosci 1997, 9: 835-847
    [90] Rubia K, Russel T and Taylor E. Brain activation in schizophrenia during performance of a Go-no-go task in fMRI, Schizophrenia Res., 1998, 1998: 112-113
    [91] Smith AM, Kiehl KA, Mendrek A, Forster BB, Hare RD and Liddle PF. Whole brain fMRI of a Go/No-go task, NeuroImage, 1998, 7: S971-S971
    [92] Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Sekihara K and Miyashita Y. No-go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging, Eur J Neurosci, 1998, 10: 1209-1213
    [93] Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kikyo H, Kameyama M and Miyashita Y. Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI, Brain, 1999, 122: 981-991
    [94] Kiehl KA, Liddle PF and Hopfinger J. Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study, Psychophysiology, 2000, 37: 216-223
    [95] Liddle PF, Kiehl KA and Smith AM. Event-related fMRI study of response inhibition, Hum Brain Mapp, 2001, 12: 100-109
    [96] Rosen AC, Rao SM, Caffarra P, Scaglioni A, Bobholz JA, Woodley SJ, Hammeke TA, Cunningham JM, Prieto TE and Binder JR. Neural basis of endogenous and exogenous spatial orienting. A functional MRI study, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1999, 11: 135-152
    [97] Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP and Shulman GL. Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex, Nature Neuroscience, 2000, 3: 292-297
    [98] Corbetta M and Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus driven attention in the brain, Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 2002, 3: 201-215
    [99] Koski LM, Paus T and Petrides M. Directed attention after unilateral frontal excisions in humans, Neurophchologia, 1998, 36: 1363 -1371
    [100] Small DM, Gitelman DR, Gregory MD, Nobre AC, Parrish TB and Mesulam MM. The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention, NeuroImage, 2003, 18: 633-641
    [101] Abrams RA and Dobikin RS. Inhibition of return: Effects of attentional cuing on eye movement latencies, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 1994, 20: 467-477
    [102] Alivisatos B. The role of the frontal cortex in the use of advance information in a mental rotation paradigm, Neuropsychologia, 1992, 30: 145-149
    [103] Klein RM. Orienting and inhibition of return, In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The newest cognitive neurosciences (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2004
    [104] Danziger S and Kingstone A. Unmasking the inhibition of return phenomenon, Perception & Psychophysics, 1999, 61: 1024-1037
    [105] Lupiá?ez J, Milliken B and Solano C. On the strategic modulation of the time course of facilitation and inhibition of return, Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Exp. Psychol, 2001, 54A: 753-773
    [106] Van der Lubbe RHJ, Maarten MH and Evelijne MB. Task-dependent exogenous cuing effects depend on cue modality, Psychphysiology, 2006, 43:145-160
    [107] Chica AB, Lupianez J and Bartolomeo P. Dissociating inhibition of return from endogenous orienting of spatial attention: Evidence from detection and discrimination tasks, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2006, 23: 1015-1034
    [108] Berger A, Henik A and Rafal R. Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention, Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 2005, 134: 207-221
    [109] Lupiá?ez J, Decaix, C, Sieroff E, Chokron S, Milliken B and Bartolomeo P. Independent effects of endogenous and exogenous spatial cueing: inhibition of return at endogenously attended target locations, Experimental Brain Research, 2004, 159: 447-457
    [110] Kiehl KA, Laurens KR, Duty TL, Forster BB and Liddle PF. Neural sorces involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: an event-related Fmri stuy, Psychophysiology, 2001, 38: 133-142

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700