用户名: 密码: 验证码:
汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的眼动研究
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
当前对汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的眼动研究相对较少,对该群体的眼动控制特征的深入研究更少。汉语发展性阅读障碍群体内部存在较大的异质性,因此还需要对不同亚类型阅读障碍群体的眼动特征进行深入分析。本研究采用眼动记录技术对汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童以及生理年龄匹配组和阅读水平匹配组儿童进行比较研究,以揭示汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的眼动特征以及阅读发展模式。共包括两个研究。
     研究一:包括一个预实验和三个正式实验,主要从阅读者获取信息的范围和类型方面探讨发展性阅读障碍儿童眼动控制的空间特征,以及从阅读者获取信息的时间进程方面探讨该类群体眼动控制的时间特征。
     预实验1筛查汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童。采用《小学生汉字识字量测试》、阅读理解测试和瑞文标准推理测验对390名小学三、四、五年级学生进行测试。根据测试成绩从四、五年级学生中筛查20名阅读障碍儿童,同时选择生理年龄匹配组儿童20名和阅读水平匹配组儿童20名。
     实验1采用移动窗口范式对汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的阅读知觉广度进行研究,同时与生理年龄匹配组和阅读水平匹配组儿童进行比较。结果发现阅读障碍儿童的阅读知觉广度为注视点右侧大约1-2个汉字的空间,精细信息的加工广度与阅读水平匹配组一致,约为1个汉字空间,粗略信息的加工广度约为2个汉字空间,小于阅读水平匹配组儿童(约3个汉字空间);年龄匹配组儿童的阅读知觉广度右侧范围明显大于阅读障碍组儿童,为注视点右侧大约2-3个汉字的空间。
     实验2采用边界范式对发展性阅读障碍儿童的预视效应进行研究,同时与生理年龄匹配组和阅读水平匹配组儿童进行比较。结果发现阅读障碍儿童和阅读水平匹配组儿童一样只在凝视时间和回视路径阅读时间两个指标上表现出字形预视效应,年龄匹配组儿童能较稳定地在副中央窝预视中获得字形信息,并能获得少量的语音预视信息。
     实验3采用消失文本范式,设置4种文本呈现时间(40ms、80ms、120ms、正常文本)研究阅读障碍儿童的信息提取速度,并与两个匹配组相比较。结果发现,阅读障碍儿童的信息提取速度最慢,大约需要120ms时间,阅读水平匹配组至少需要80ms时间,年龄匹配组儿童的信息提取时间大约在40-80ms。当文本呈现时间较短时,各组儿童都能相应地改变阅读策略以适应这种变化,具体表现为延长注视时间、增加注视次数,减小向右眼跳幅度,同时降低再注视比率。
     研究二:对主要发展性阅读障碍亚类型儿童在时间和空间两个维度上的眼动控制特征进行比较研究,包括一个预实验和三个正式实验。
     预实验2对汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的主要亚类型进行分析。采用预实验1的测量方法从另外519名四、五年级的学生中筛查37名阅读障碍儿童。然后对所有阅读障碍儿童和阅读水平匹配组儿童进行语音意识、语素意识、正字法意识和快速命名测验。最后采用标准差切分法,以阅读水平匹配组儿童的测验成绩为参照,对阅读障碍儿童进行亚类型分析。结果发现,语音意识缺陷人数比例最大,其次是正字法意识缺陷,混合缺陷中以语音正字法混合缺陷为主。最终确定将单纯语音意识缺陷型、单纯正字法意识缺陷型、语音正字法混合缺陷型和轻微缺陷型作为本研究的主要阅读障碍亚类型。
     实验4对主要阅读障碍亚类型儿童的阅读知觉广度进行研究,并与生理年龄匹配组和阅读水平匹配组儿童进行比较。结果发现,语音意识缺陷组和正字法缺陷组儿童的阅读知觉广度与阅读水平匹配组基本相同,约为注视点右侧1-2个汉字的空间,语音正字法意识混合缺陷组儿童的知觉广度最小,约为注视点右侧1个汉字的空间,轻微缺陷组儿童的阅读知觉广度与年龄匹配组儿童大致相当,为注视点右侧2-3个汉字的空间。
     实验5对主要阅读障碍亚类型儿童的预视效应进行研究。结果发现轻微缺陷组儿童能从副中央窝处获得字形预视信息,但是在阅读的早期阶段预视作用不太稳定;单一缺陷组儿童仅在凝视时间指标上存在预视效应,语音意识缺陷组能获得字形预视信息,正字法意识缺陷组儿童能获得语音预视信息;语音正字法意识混合缺陷型儿童既不能获得字形预视信息也不能获得语音预视信息。
     实验6对主要阅读障碍亚类型儿童的阅读信息提取时间进行研究。结果发现,语音意识缺陷型儿童和正字法意识缺陷型儿童对阅读信息的提取时间大约需要120ms。当文本呈现时间较短时,两组儿童对阅读策略的调整方式也比较一致。轻微缺陷型儿童阅读信息的提取时间大约为80ms,在文本呈现较短时间时,阅读策略的调整与匹配组儿童基本一致。语音正字法混合缺陷型儿童对阅读信息的提取至少需要120ms时间。
     综合以上研究,得出如下结论:(1)阅读障碍儿童群体的眼动控制特征与阅读水平匹配组儿童存在较小差异,但与年龄匹配组儿童相比,发展水平较低。(2)阅读障碍亚类型间的发展水平存在差异,其中轻微缺陷型儿童与年龄匹配组儿童差异较小;单一缺陷型儿童的信息获取范围和预视效应量与阅读水平匹配组无明显差异,但信息提取速度较慢于阅读水平匹配组;语音正字法混合缺陷型儿童的阅读受损程度最严重,眼动控制特征明显差于阅读水平匹配组儿童。
In current study eye-tracker was used to explore the oculomotor characters and reading development model of the developmental dyslexia with their age-matched and ability-matched children. Two preliminary experiments and six experiments were included in this study.
     The purpose of preliminary experiment1was to screen Chinese developmental dyslexia. The assessment of vocabulary of Chinese characters for primary school (Wang&Tao,1996), the test of Chinese reading comprehension (Leong&Tse,2008) and Raven Standard Progressive Matrices Test were used to test390primary pupils. Then we selected20developmental dyslexia and their age-matched children from forth-and fifth-graders and20reading ability-matched children from third-graders.
     In experiment1, the moving window paradigm was used to explore the perceptual span of the developmental dyslexic children and their age-matched and ability-matched children. The results showed that the right perceptual span of dyslexics was about one to two Chinese characters. The fine information processing span of dyslexics was about one Chinese character which was similar to that of their ability-matched children. On the other hand, the gross information processing span of dyslexics which was about two Chinese characters was smaller than that of their ability-matched children which was about three Chinese characters. This indicated that the ability of planning saccade of dyslexics was relatively poorer than their ability-matched children. The right span of age-matched children was distinctly larger than that of dyslexics. The fine information processing span of ability-matched children was approximately two Chinese characters and that of gross information was about three Chinese characters.
     In experiment2, the boundary paradigm was used to explore the preview effects of the developmental dyslexic children and their matched children. The results found that dyslexics and their ability-matched ones could acquire orthographic preview benefits during analysis of gaze duration and regression path reading time while age-matched children could obtain strong orthographic preview benefits and a little phonological preview benefits.
     In experiment3, the disappearing text paradigm was adopted. Four text appearing time(40ms、80ms、120ms、normal text) was set to explore the speed of capturing visual information of the developmental dyslexic children and their matched children. The results showed that the speed of capturing information of the dyslexic was about120ms which was the slowest. The speed of the ability-matched children was about80ms while that of the age-matched children was40-80ms. All children could accordingly change their reading strategy when the text appearing time was short.
     Preliminary experiment2was undertaken to analyze the main subtypes of the Chinese developmental dyslexia. Thirty-seven dyslexics were selected from519fourth-and fifth-graders with the same means used in preliminary experiment1. And then four reading-related cognitive skills were examined, including phonological awareness, morphological awareness, orthographic awareness and rapid automatic naming. Based on the scores of ability-match children standard deviation cutoff criterion was adopted to analyze the subtypes of dyslexia. The results showed that the proportion of the dyslexics with phonological awareness deficit was the largest and secondly the dyslexics with orthographic awareness deficit. Finally, the dyslexics with single phonological awareness deficit, single orthographic awareness deficit, and phonology-orthographic awareness mixed deficit and slight deficit were defined as the main subtypes of dyslexia in current study.
     Experiment4was carried out to investigate the perceptual span of main subtypes of dyslexia. The results found that the right perceptual span of the dyslexics with single phonological awareness deficit and single orthographic awareness deficit was about one to two Chinese characters which were similar with that of ability-matched children. The perceptual span of dyslexics with phonology-orthographic awareness mixed deficit was the most small which was about one Chinese character to the right of fixation. The right perceptual span of the dyslexics with slight deficit was about two to three Chinese characters which was similar with that of age-matched children.
     In experiment5, the preview effects of main subtypes of dyslexia were explored. The results were as follows:the dyslexics with slight deficit could obtain orthographic information from parafoveal whereas the preview effect was not stable during the early stage of reading process. The dyslexics with single deficit could acquire preview effects only during analysis of gaze duration while the dyslexics with single phonological deficit could receive orthographic information from parafoveal vision and the dyslexics with single orthographic deficit receive phonological information. The dyslexics with mixed deficit could not obtain phonological or orthographic preview benefits.
     Experiment6was conducted to investigate the speed of acquiring information of the main subtypes of dyslexia. The results found that the speed of acquiring information of dyslexics with single deficit was about120ms. The speed of dyslexics with slight deficit was about80ms while their reading strategies were similar with matched children when the very short time of text presentation. The dyslexics with mixed deficit need at least120ms time to obtain reading information.
     In summary, the conclusions are as follows:(1) the oculormotor characteristics of dyslexics were not different from their ability-matched children whereas behind of age-matched children.(2) There were differences between the main subtypes of dyslexia. The discrepancy was on the small side between the dyslexics with slight deficit and their age-matched children. The speed of acquiring information of the dyslexics with single deficit was slower than that of ability-matched children. The development of oculormotor of dyslexics with mixed deficit was behind of ability-matched ones.
引文
白学军,孟红霞,王敬欣,田静,臧传丽,闫国利.(2011).阅读障碍儿童与其年龄和阅读水平匹配儿童阅读空格文本的注视位置效应.心理学报,43(8),851-862.
    白学军,刘娟,臧传丽,张慢慢,郭晓峰,闫国利.(2011).中文阅读过程中的副中央凹预视效应.心理科学进展,19(12),1721-1729.
    陈洪波,杨志伟,唐效兰.(2001).汉语阅读障碍儿童的认知能力[Ⅰ][而].中国心理卫生杂志16(1).
    陈庆荣,邓铸.(2006).阅读中的眼动控制理论与SWIFT模型.心理科学进展,14(5),675-681.
    伏干,闫国利.(2008).高中二年级学生双字词阅读知觉广度的眼动研究.心理与行为研究6(1),285-290.
    关善玲,闫国利.(2007).移动窗口条件下不同工作记忆者阅读差异的眼动研究.心理与行为研究,5(4),309-313.
    郝献忠,田玉红.(2008).汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的矫治训练.中国全科医学,5(9).
    胡笑羽,白学军,闫国利.(2010).副中央凹—中央凹效应的研究现状及展望.心理科学进展,18(3),412-420.
    黄旭,吴汉荣,静进,邹小兵,王梦龙,李秀红等.(2007).汉语阅读障碍儿童在快速命名时的眼动特征.中国心理卫生杂志,21(6),358-361.
    姜涛,彭聃龄.(1999).汉语儿童的语音意识特点及阅读能力高低读者的差异.心理学报,31(1).
    李虹,彭虹,舒华.(2006).汉语儿童正字法意识的萌芽与发展.心理发展与教育,22(1),35-38.
    李秀红,静进,邹小兵等.(2007).汉语阅读障碍儿童阅读文章的眼动试验研究.儿童心理卫生,21(4),362-374.
    刘文理,刘翔平.(2006).阅读发展相关的认知技能:汉语和英语的比较.心理科学进展,14(9).
    刘文理,刘翔平,张婧乔.(2006).汉语发展性阅读障碍亚类型的初步探讨.心理学报,38(5),681-693.
    刘志方,张智君,赵亚军.(2011).汉语阅读中眼跳目标选择单元以及词汇加工方式:来自消失文本的实验证据.心理学报,43(6),608-618.
    刘志方,闫国利,白学军.(2011).消失文本词窗口条件下八年级学生阅读的眼动特征.心理研究,4(6),26-29.
    刘志方.(2013).中文阅读中的词切分与词汇加工方式.浙江大学博士论文.
    刘志方,闫国利,张智君,潘运,杨桂芳.(2013).中文阅读中的预视效应与词切分.心理学报.45(6).
    孟祥芝.(2001).汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的汉字表征与加工.北京师范大学博士学位论文
    钱怡,赵婧,毕鸿燕.(2013).汉语学龄前儿童正字法意识的发展.心理学报,45(1),60-69.
    吴思娜,舒华,王彧.(2004).4-6年级小学生发展性阅读障碍的异质性研究.心理发展与教育,(3),46-50.
    吴思娜,舒华,刘艳茹.(2005).语素意识在儿童汉语阅读中的作用.心理与行为研究,3(3),35-38.
    吴思娜.(2004).汉语发展性阅读障碍亚类型.北京师范大学博士论文.
    王孝玲,陶保平.(1996).小学生识字量测试题库及评价量表.上海教育出版社.
    王艳碧,余林.(2007).我国近十年来汉语阅读障碍研究回顾与展望.心理科学进展,15(4):596-604.
    舒华,毕雪梅,武宁宁.(2003).声旁部分信息在儿童学习和记忆汉字中的作用.心理学报,35,9-16.
    隋雪,姜娜,钱丽.(2010).汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童词汇阅读的眼动研究,中国特殊教育,117(3),63-67.
    隋雪,刘雯,翁旭初.(2012).汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童语素分辨及运用能力.辽宁师范大学学报:社会科学版(1).
    王丽红,王永妍,闫国利.(2010).词汇获得年龄效应的眼动研究.心理与行为研究,8(12).
    王敬欣,杨洪艳,田静.(2010).发展性阅读障碍儿童语篇阅读中的笔画数效应.中国特殊教育,(5),36-40.
    王穗苹,佟秀红,杨锦棉,冷英.(2009).中文句子阅读中语义信息对眼动预视效应的影响.心理学报,41(3),220-232.
    王晓辰.(2010).汉语发展性阅读障碍语音及正字法缺损的认知过程基础—基于PASS理论的研究.华东师范大学博士学位论文.
    熊建萍,闫国利,白学军.(2007).高中二年级学生中文阅读知觉广度的眼动研究.心理与行为研究,5(1),60-64.
    熊建萍,闫国利,白学军.(2009).不同年级学生汉语阅读知觉广度的眼动研究.心理科学,32,584-587.
    薛锦,舒华,杨剑锋,陆建平.(2008).发展性阅读障碍亚类型研究中的问题.中国特殊教育,93(3),48-52.
    闫国利,白学军.(2000).中文阅读过程的眼动研究.心理学动态,8(3),19-22.
    闫国利,伏干,白学军.(2008).不同难度阅读材料对阅读知觉广度影响的眼动研究.心理科学,31,1287-1290.
    闫国利,胡晏雯,刘志方,张智君.(2009).消失文本条件下注视点右侧词对中文阅读影响的眼动研究.应用心理学,5(3),201-207.
    闫国利,王丽红,巫金根,白学军.(2011).不同年级学生阅读知觉广度及预视效益的眼动研究.心理学报,43,249-263.
    闫国利,巫金根,胡晏雯,白学军.(2010).当前阅读的眼动研究范式述评.心理科学进属18(2),1966-1976.
    闫国利,王文静,白学军.(2007).消失文本条件下认知控制的眼动研究.心理学探新,27(4),37-41.
    闫国利,巫金根,臧传丽,白学军.(2010).阅读知觉广度眼动研究述评.心理学探新,30(2),23-28.
    闫国利,熊建萍,白学军.(2008).小学五年级学生汉语阅读知觉广度的眼动研究.心理发展与教育,24(1),72-77.
    闫国利,熊建萍,臧传丽,余莉莉,崔磊,白学军.(2013).阅读研究中的主要眼动指标评述.心理科学进展,21,1-17.
    闫国利,张巧明,白学军.(2013).中文阅读知觉广度的影响因素研究.心理发展与教育,29(3).
    杨志伟,龚耀先,李雪荣.(1998).汉语儿童阅读障碍的临床评定与分型研究.中国临床·理学杂志,6(3),136-139.
    臧传丽,张慢慢,岳音其,白学军,闫国利.(2013).副中央凹信息量对中文朗读和默读的调节作用.心理与行为研究,11(7).
    曾性初,张履祥,王家柱.(1965).汉语的讯息分析-Ⅰ.文句中汉字笔画的省略与恢复.心理学报,10(4),299-306.
    张承芬等.(1998).汉语阅读困难儿童认知特征研究.心理学报,30(1),50-55.
    张巧明.(2013).小学生阅读知觉广度与速度的发展:来自眼动的证据.天津师范大学博士论文.
    张巧明,王爱云,闫国利.(2013).大学生阅读知觉广度影响因素的回归分析.心理与行为研 究,11(3).
    张仙峰,叶文玲.(2006).当前阅读研究中眼动指标述评.心理与行为为研究,4(3),236-240.
    Ashby, J., Treiman, R., Kessler, B., & Rayner, K. (2006).Vowel processing during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, memory, and Cognition,32(2),416-424.
    Bai X., Zang C., Yan G., & Shen D. (2006, May). The role of different linguistic codes in parafoveal processing. The 2nd China Internal Conference on Eye Movements, China, Tianjin.
    Bruck, M. (1992).Persistence of dyslexics'phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology.28,874-886.
    Badian, N. A. (1997). Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis. Annals of Dyslexia,47,69-87.
    Badian, N. A.(2001).Phonological and orthographic processing:their roles in reading prediction. Annals of Dyslexia,51,179-202.
    Chen, K. J., & Liu, S. H. (1992). Word identification for Mandarin Chinese sentences. Proceedings of the Fifteenth International Conference on Computational Linguistics, Nantes: COLING-92.
    Chen X, Shu H, Wu N N, Anderson R. C. (2003). Stages in learning to pronounce Chinese characters. Psychology in the Schools,40(1),115-124.
    Chen, H-C., Song, H., Lau, W. Y., Wong, K. F. E., & Tang, S. L.(2003). Development characteristics of eye movement in reading Chinese. In C. McBride- Chang & H-C. Chen (Eds.), Chinese children's reading development (pp.157-169). New Haven, CT:Greenwood.
    Chen, H.-C., & Tang, C.K. (1998). The effective visual field in reading Chinese. Reading and Writing,10,3-5.
    Drieghe, D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2005). Eye movements and word skipping during reading revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 31(5),954-969.
    Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E. M., & Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT:A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review,112,777-813.
    Flores d'Arcais, G. B. (1994). Order of strokes writing as a cue for retrieval in reading Chinese characters. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,6,337-355.
    Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia:Towards a cross linguistic framework. Dyslexia,6,133-151.
    Johnson, R. L., & Rayner, K. (2007). Top-down and bottom-up effects in pure alexia:Evidence from eye movements. Neuropsychology,45,2246-2257.
    Haikio, T., Bertram R., Hyona, J., & Niemi, P. (2009). Development of the letter identity span in reading:Evidence from the eye movement moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,102,167-181.
    Hann, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading, and dyslexia:Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review,106,491-528.
    Ho C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. (1997). Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese. Developmental Psychology,33,946-951.
    Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W., Tsang, S. M., & Lee, S. H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese development dyslexia. Journal of Developmental Psychology,38,543-553.
    Ho C. S-H, Chan D. W-O, Lee S-H, Tsang S-M, Luan V H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition,91,43-75.
    Ho, C. S. H., Law, T. P. S.,& NG, P. M. (2000). The phonological deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing,13(1),57-79.
    Hohenstein, S., Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2010). Semantic preview benefit in eye movements during reading:A parafoveal fast-priming study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,36(5),1150-1170.
    Inhoff, A. W., & Liu, W. (1998). The perceptual span and oculomotor activity during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance,24,20-34.
    Inhoff, A. W., & Radach, R. (1998). Definition and computation of oculormotor measures in the study of cognitive processes. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp.29-53). Oxford:Oxford University Press.
    Inhoff, A. W., Starr, M., Liu, W. M., & Wang, J. (1998).Eye-movement-contingent display changes are not compromised by flicker and phosphor persistence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,5,101-106.
    Irwin, D. E. (1998). Lexical processing during saccadic eye movements. Cognitive Psychology,36,1-27.
    Leong, C. K., Loh, K. Y., Ki, W. W., & Tse, S. K. (2011). Enhancing orthographic knowledge helps spelling production in eight-year-old Chinese children at risk for dyslexia. Annual of Dyslexia,61(1),136-160.
    Leong, C.K., Tse, S. K. (2008). Text comprehension in Chinese children:relative contribution of verbal working memory, pseudoword reading, rapid automatized naming, and onset-rime phonological segmentation. Journal of Educational Psychology,100:135-149.
    Liversedge, S. P., Paterson, K. B., & Pickering, M. J. (1998). Eye movements and measures of reading time. In Underwood, G. (Ed.) Eye guidance in reading and scene perception(pp.55-77). Oxford:Oxford University Press.
    Lyon, G. R.(1995).Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia,45,3-27.
    Lyon, G.R.,& Shaywitz, S.E.(2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia,53,1-14.
    Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Stallings, L., Joanisse, M., Bailey, C., Freedman, L., Curtin, S.& Keating, P. (1999). Development of dyslexic subgroups:A one-year follow up. Annals of Dyslexia.49,105-134.
    McConkie, G. W. & Rayner, K. (1975). The span of the effective stimulus during a fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics,17,578-586.
    Morris, R.D., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., et al. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability: variability around a phonological core. Journal of Educational Psychology,90(3),347-373.
    O'Regan, J. K. (1990). Eye movement and reading. In Kowler(Ed.). Reviews of oculormotor research (Vol.4). Eye Movements and Their Role in Visual and Cognitive Processes (pp.395-453). Amsterdam:Elsevier.
    O'Regan, J. K. (1992). Optimal viewing position in words and the strategy-tactics theory of eye movements in reading. In K. Rayner(Ed.), Eye Movements and Visual Cognition:Scene Perception and Reading (pp.333-354). New York:Springer.
    Perfetti, C. A., Tan, L. H., Siok, W. T. (2006). Brain-behavior relations in reading and dyslexia: implications of Chinese results. Brain and Language.98,344-346.
    Pollatsek, A., Reichle, E. D., & Rayner, K. (2006). Tests of the E-Z Reader model:Exploring the interface between cognition and eye-movement control. Cognitive Psychology,52,1-56.
    Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J. & Olson, R. K (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia:A review. Reading Research Quarterly,27,29-53.
    Roberts, R., & Mather, N. (1997).orthographic dyslexia:The neglected subtype, Learning Disabilities Research & Practice,12,236-250.
    Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review,105,125-157.
    Reichle, E.D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E-Z Reader model of eye-movement control in reading:Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,26,445-476.
    Reilly, R. & O'Regan, J. K. (1998). Eye-movement control in reading:a simulation of some word-targeting strategies. Visual Research,38,303-317.
    Richter, E.M., Engbert, R., and Kliegl, R. (2006). Current advances in SWIFT. Cognitive Systems Research,7,23-33.
    Sereno, S. C. & Rayner, K. (1992). Fast priming during eye fixation in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance,18(1),173-184.
    Siegel, L. S. (1993). The cognitive basis of dyslexia. Emerging themes in cognitive development. New York:Springer Verlag,33-52.
    Shen, D, Bai, X, Yan, G, & Liversedge, S.P. (2008). The perceptual span in Chinese reading. In K. Rayner, D. Shen, X. Bai, & G. Yan (Eds.). Cognitive and cultural influences on eye movements (pp.255-276). Tianjin:Tianjin People's Publishing House.
    Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia:Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology,98,122-133.
    Stevenson, H. W., Stigler, J. W., Lucker, G. W., Hsu, C. C. & Kitamura, S. (1982). Reading disabilities:the case of Chinese, Japanese, and English. Child Development,53,1164-1181.
    Sun, F., & Feng, D. (1999). Eye movements in reading Chinese and English text. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script:A cognitive analysis (pp.189-206). Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum.
    Swan, D., & Goswami, U.(1997) Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,66,18-41.
    Tan, L. H., John, A. S., Guinevere, F. E., et al. (2005). Reading depends on writing in Chinese. PNAS.102(24),8781-8785.
    Tong, X., McBride-Chang, C.; Shu H.; Wong, A. M-Y. (2009). Morphological awareness, orthographic knowledge and spelling errors:keys to understanding early Chinese literacy acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading,13 (5),426-452.
    Tsang, Y. K., & Chen, H. C. (2008). Eye movements in reading Chinese. In K. Rayner., D. Shen, X. Bai, & G. Yan (Eds.), Cognitive and cultural influences on eye movements (pp.235-254). Tianjin:Tianjin People's Publishing House.
    Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. Y., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Yen, N. S. (2004). Use of phonological codes for Chinese characters:Evidence from processing of parafoveal preview when reading sentences. Brain and Language,91,235-244.
    Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999).The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology,91,415-438.
    Wolf, M., Orourke, A. G, & Gidney C., et. al. (2002). The second deficit:An investigation of independence of phonological and naming speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing:an Interdisciplinary Journal,15:43--72.
    Wang, S., Yang, J., & Chen, H. C. (2008). Immediacy of processing intra-sentential and inter-sentential information in reading Chinese. In K. Rayner, D. Shen, X. Bai, & G. Yan (Eds.). Cognitive and cultural influences on eye movements (pp.315-341). Tianjin:Tianjin People's Publishing House.
    Yan, G., Bai, X., Zang, C., Bian, Q., Cui, L., Qi, W., Rayner, K., & Liversedge, S.P. (2012). Using stroke removal to investigate Chinese character identification during reading:Evidence from eye movements. Reading and Writing,25(5),951-979.
    Yan, M., Richter, E. M., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2009). Readers of Chinese extract semantic information from parafoveal words. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,16,561-566.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700