Directed forgetting benefits motor sequence encoding
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Tobias Tempel ; Christian Frings
  • 关键词:Body movement ; Recall ; Directed forgetting ; Motor learning
  • 刊名:Memory & Cognition
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:April 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:44
  • 期:3
  • 页码:413-419
  • 全文大小:387 KB
  • 参考文献:Ascoli, K. M., & Schmidt, R. A. (1969). Proactive interference in short-term motor retention. Journal of Motor Behavior, 1, 29–35. doi:10.​1080/​00222895.​1969.​10734831 CrossRef PubMed
    Bjork, R. A. (1972). Theoretical implications of directed forgetting. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 217–235). Washington, DC: Winston.
    Bjork, R. A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In H. L. Roediger III & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 309–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Brashers-Krug, T., Shadmehr, R., & Bizzi, E. (1996). Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature, 382, 252–255. doi:10.​1038/​382252a0 CrossRef PubMed
    Burwitz, L. (1974). Proactive interference and directed forgetting in short-term motor memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 799–805. doi:10.​1037/​h0036361 CrossRef
    Cothros, N., Köhler, S., Dickie, E. W., Mirsattari, S. M., & Gribble, P. L. (2006). Proactive interference as a result of persisting neural representations of previously learned motor skills in primary motor cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 2167–2176. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2006.​18.​12.​2167 CrossRef PubMed
    Earles, J. L., & Kersten, A. W. (2002). Directed forgetting of actions by younger and older adults. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 383–388. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196297 CrossRef
    Irtel, H. (2007). PXLab: The Psychological Experiments Laboratory [online]. Version 2.1.11. Mannheim (Germany): University of Mannheim. Available from <http://​www.​pxlab.​de >
    MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104. doi:10.​1037/​1082-989X.​7.​1.​83 CrossRef PubMed PubMedCentral
    Panzer, S., Wilde, H., & Shea, C. H. (2006). Learning of similar complex movement sequences: Proactive and retroactive effects on learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38, 60–70. doi:10.​3200/​JMBR.​38.​1.​60-70 CrossRef PubMed
    Pastötter, B., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2010). Amount of postcue encoding predicts amount of directed forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 54–65. doi:10.​1037/​a0017406 PubMed
    Pastötter, B., Kliegl, O., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2012). List-method directed forgetting: The forget cue improves both encoding and retrieval of postcue information. Memory & Cognition, 40, 861–873. doi:10.​3758/​s13421-012-0206-4 CrossRef
    Reppa, I., Worth, E. R., Greville, W. J., & Saunders, J. (2013). The representation of response effector and response location in episodic memory for newly acquired actions: Evidence from retrieval-induced forgetting. Acta Psychologica, 143, 210–217. doi:10.​1016/​j.​actpsy.​2013.​03.​007 CrossRef PubMed
    Sahakyan, L., & Delaney, P. F. (2003). Can encoding differences explain the benefits of directed forgetting in the list-method paradigm? Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 195–201. doi:10.​1016/​S0749-596X(02)00524-7 CrossRef
    Sahakyan, L., & Foster, N. L. (2009). Intentional forgetting of actions: Comparison of list-method and item-method directed forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 134–152. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jml.​2009.​02.​006 CrossRef
    Sahakyan, L., & Kelley, C. M. (2002). A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 1064–1072. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​28.​6.​1064 PubMed
    Tempel, T., & Frings, C. (2013). Resolving interference between body movements: Retrieval-induced forgetting of motor sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1152–1161. doi:10.​1037/​a0030336 PubMed
    Tempel, T., & Frings, C. (2014a). Interference within hands: Retrieval-induced forgetting of left and right hand movements. Acta Psychologica, 148, 1–5. doi:10.​1016/​j.​actpsy.​2014.​01.​003 CrossRef PubMed
    Tempel, T., & Frings, C. (2014b). Forgetting motor programmes: Retrieval dynamics in procedural memory. Memory, 22, 1116–1125. doi:10.​1080/​09658211.​2013.​871293 CrossRef PubMed
    Tempel, T., & Frings, C. (2015). Categorization by movement direction: Retrieval-induced forgetting of motor sequences grouped by motion features. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 473–486. doi:10.​1080/​17470218.​2014.​945098 CrossRef
    Tempel, T., Loran, I., & Frings, C. (2015). Dancing your moves away: How memory retrieval shapes complex motor action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. doi:10.​1037/​xap0000052 PubMed
    Williams, I. D. (1971). The effects of practice trials and prior learning on motor memory. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3, 205–212. doi:10.​1080/​00222895.​1971.​10734903 CrossRef PubMed
    Wohldmann, E. L., Healy, A. F., & Bourne, L. E., Jr. (2008). A mental practice superiority effect: Less retroactive interference and more transfer than physical practice. Journal Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 823–833. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​34.​4.​823
  • 作者单位:Tobias Tempel (1)
    Christian Frings (1)

    1. Department of Psychology, University of Trier, Trier, 54286, Germany
  • 刊物主题:Cognitive Psychology;
  • 出版者:Springer US
  • ISSN:1532-5946
文摘
Two experiments investigated directed forgetting of newly learned motor sequences. Concurrently with the list method of directed forgetting, participants successively learned two lists of motor sequences. Each sequence consisted of four consecutive finger movements. After a short distractor task, a recall test was given. Both experiments compared a forget group that was instructed to forget list-1 items with a remember group not receiving a forget instruction. We found that the instruction to forget list 1 enhanced recall of subsequently learned motor sequences. This benefit of directed forgetting occurred independently of costs for list 1. A mediation analysis showed that the encoding accuracy of list 2 was a mediator of the recall benefit, that is, the more accurate execution of motor sequences of list 2 after receiving a forget instruction for list 1 accounted for better recall of list 2. Thus, the adaptation of the list method to motor action provided more direct evidence on the effect of directed forgetting on subsequent learning. The results corroborate the assumption of a reset of encoding as a consequence of directed forgetting.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700