Aesthetic appreciation of poetry correlates with ease of processing in event-related potentials
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Christian Obermeier ; Sonja A. Kotz…
  • 关键词:Neuroaesthetics ; Poetry ; ERP ; N400 ; P600
  • 刊名:Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:April 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:16
  • 期:2
  • 页码:362-373
  • 全文大小:1,403 KB
  • 参考文献:Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419–439. doi:10.​1006/​jmla.​1997.​2558 CrossRef
    Aristotle. (1932). The poetics. In G. P. Goold (Ed.), Aristotle/Longinus/Demetrius, “The poetics”/“On the sublime”/“On style” (pp. 3–117). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
    Barrett, S. E., & Rugg, M. D. (1990). Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures. Brain and Cognition, 14, 201–212.CrossRef PubMed
    Berlyne, D. E. (1974). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics. Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. Washington: Hemisphere.
    Bohn, K., Knaus, J., Wiese, R., & Domahs, U. (2013). The influence of rhythmic (ir)regularities on speech processing: Evidence from an ERP study on German phrases. Neuropsychologia, 51, 760–771. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuropsychologia​.​2013.​01.​006 CrossRef PubMed
    Brattico, E., & Jacobsen, T. (2009). Subjective appraisal of music: Neuroimaging evidence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 308–317.CrossRef PubMed
    Brown, S., Gao, X., Tisdelle, L., Eickhoff, S. B., & Liotti, M. (2011). Naturalizing aesthetics: Brain areas for aesthetic appraisal across sensory modalities. NeuroImage, 58, 250–258.CrossRef PubMed
    Bullot, N. J., & Reber, R. (2013). The artful mind meets art history: Toward a psycho-historical framework for the science of art appreciation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 123–137.CrossRef PubMed
    Cela-Conde, C. J., García-Prieto, J., Ramasco, J. J., Mirasso, C. R., Bajo, R., Munar, E., … Maestú, F. (2013). Dynamics of brain networks in the aesthetic appreciation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 10454–10461.
    Chatterjee, A., Thomas, A., Smith, S. E., & Aguirre, G. K. (2009). The neural response to facial attractiveness. Neuropsychology, 23, 135–143.CrossRef PubMed
    Chatterjee, A., & Vartanian, O. (2014). Neuroaesthetics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 370–375. doi:10.​1016/​j.​tics.​2014.​03.​003 CrossRef PubMed
    Coch, D., Grossi, G., Skendzel, W., & Neville, H. (2005). ERP nonword rhyming effects in children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 168–182.CrossRef PubMed
    Coch, D., Hart, T., & Mitra, P. (2008). Three kinds of rhymes: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 104, 230–243.CrossRef PubMed
    Cross, E. S., Kirsch, L., Ticini, L. F., & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2011). The impact of aesthetic evaluation and physical ability on dance perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 102. doi:10.​3389/​fnhum.​2011.​00102 CrossRef PubMed PubMedCentral
    Cutler, A., & Foss, D. J. (1977). On the role of sentence stress in sentence processing. Language and Speech, 20, 1–10.PubMed
    Davids, N., van den Brink, D., van Turennout, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2011). Task-related influences on neurophysiological assessment of auditory rhyme: Implications for clinical studies. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 1629–1636. doi:10.​1016/​j.​clinph.​2010.​12.​058 CrossRef PubMed
    Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Berkeley.
    Essens, P. J., & Povel, D.-J. (1985). Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 37, 1–7. doi:10.​3758/​BF03207132 CrossRef
    Fabb, N. (1997). Linguistics and literature: Language in the verbal arts of the world. Oxford: Blackwell.
    Fechner, G. T. (1876). Vorschule der Aesthetik [Elements of aesthetics]. Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel.
    Fitch, W. T., von Graevenitz, A., & Nicolas, E. (2009). Bio-aesthetics and the aesthetic trajectory: A dynamic cognitive and cultural perspective. In M. Skov & O. Vartanian (Eds.), Neuroaesthetics (pp. 59–102). Amityville: Baywood.
    Galak, J., & Nelson, L. D. (2011). The virtues of opaque prose: How lay beliefs about fluency influence perceptions of quality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 250–253. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jesp.​2010.​08.​002 CrossRef
    Giora, R., Fein, O., Kronrod, A., Elnatan, I., Shuval, N., & Zur, A. (2004). Weapons of mass distraction: Optimal innovation and pleasure ratings. Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 115–141. doi:10.​1207/​s15327868ms1902_​2 CrossRef
    Grewe, O., Nagel, F., Kopiez, R., & Altenmüller, E. (2005). How does music arouse “chills”? Investigating strong emotions, combining psychological, physiological, and psychoacoustic methods. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060, 446–449.CrossRef PubMed
    Hekkert, P., Snelders, D., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (2003). “Most advanced, yet acceptable”: Typicality and novelty as joint predictors of aesthetic preference in industrial design. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 111–124. doi:10.​1348/​0007126037628421​47 CrossRef PubMed
    Jacobsen, T. (2013). On the electrophysiology of aesthetic processing. In S. Finger, D. W. Zaidel, F. Boller, & J. Bogousslavsky (Eds.), The fine Arts, neurology, and neuroscience: New discoveries and changing landscapes (pp. 159–168). New York: Elsevier.CrossRef
    Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R. I., Höfel, L., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. NeuroImage, 29, 276–285. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuroimage.​2005.​07.​010 CrossRef PubMed
    Jakobson, R. (1960). Closing statement: Linguistics and poetics. In T. A. Seboek (Ed.), Style in language (pp. 350–377). Cambridge: MIT Press.
    Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). How infants begin to extract words from speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 323–328. doi:10.​1016/​S1364-6613(99)01363-7 CrossRef PubMed
    Khateb, A., Pegna, A. J., Landis, T., Michel, C. M., Brunet, D., Seghier, M. L., & Annoni, J. M. (2007). Rhyme processing in the brain: An ERP mapping study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 240–250.CrossRef PubMed
    Kirsch, L. P., Drommelschmidt, K. A., & Cross, E. S. (2013). The impact of sensorimotor experience on affective evaluation of dance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 521. doi:10.​3389/​fnhum.​2013.​00521 CrossRef PubMed PubMedCentral
    Kotz, S. A., & Schwartze, M. (2010). Cortical speech processing unplugged: A timely subcortico-cortical framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 392–399.CrossRef PubMed
    Kramer, A. F., & Donchin, E. (1987). Brain potentials as indices of orthographic and phonological interaction during word matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 76–86. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​13.​1.​76 PubMed
    Kreitler, H., & Kreitler, S. (1972). Psychology of the arts. Durham: Duke University Press.
    Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159. doi:10.​1037/​0033-295X.​106.​1.​119 CrossRef
    Lea, R., Rapp, D., Elfenbein, A., Mitchel, A., & Romine, R. (2008). Sweet silent thought: Alliteration and resonance in poetry comprehension. Psychological Science, 19, 709–716.CrossRef PubMed
    Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, M. D. (2004). A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 489–508. doi:10.​1348/​0007126042369811​ CrossRef PubMed
    Luo, Y. Y., & Zhou, X. L. (2010). ERP evidence for the online processing of rhythmic pattern during Chinese sentence reading. NeuroImage, 49, 2836–2849. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuroimage.​2009.​10.​008 CrossRef PubMed
    Magne, C., Astesano, C., Aramaki, M., Ystad, S., Kronland-Martinet, R., & Besson, M. (2007). Influence of syllabic lengthening on semantic processing in spoken French: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2659–2668. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhl174 CrossRef PubMed
    Magne, C., Gordon, R. L., Midha, S. (2010). Influence of metrical expectancy on reading words: An ERP study. Paper presented at the Fifth International Conference on Speech Prosody, Chicago, IL.
    Marie, C., Magne, C., & Besson, M. (2011). Musicians and the metric structure of words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 294–305.CrossRef PubMed
    Martindale, C. (1984). The pleasures of thought: A theory of cognitive hedonics. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 5, 49–80.
    McCauley, S. M., Hestvik, A., & Vogel, I. (2013). Perception and bias in the processing of compound versus phrasal stress: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Language and Speech, 56, 23–44.CrossRef PubMed
    Menninghaus, W., Bohrn, I. C., Altmann, U., Lubrich, O., & Jacobs, A. M. (2014). Sounds funny? Humor effects of phonological and prosodic figures of speech. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 71–76. doi:10.​1037/​a0035309 CrossRef
    Menninghaus, W., Bohrn, I., Knoop, C., Kotz, S. A., Schlotz, W., & Jacobs, A. (2015). Rhetorical features facilitate prosodic processing while handicapping ease of semantic comprehension. Cognition, 143, 48–60. doi:10.​1016/​j.​cognition.​2015.​05.​026 CrossRef PubMed
    Miall, D. S., & Kuiken, D. (1994). Foregrounding, defamiliarization, and affect-response to literary stories. Poetics, 22, 389–407. doi:10.​1016/​0304-422x(94)00011-5 CrossRef
    Miall, D. S., & Kuiken, D. (1998). The form of reading: Empirical studies of literariness. Poetics, 25, 327–341.CrossRef
    Nadal, M. (2013). The experience of art: Insights from neuroimaging. Progress in Brain Research, 204, 135–158.CrossRef PubMed
    Obermeier, C., Menninghaus, W., von Koppenfels, M., Raettig, T., Schmidt-Kassow, M., Otterbein, S., & Kotz, S. A. (2013). Aesthetic and emotional effects of meter and rhyme in poetry. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 10. doi:10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2013.​00010 CrossRef PubMed PubMedCentral
    Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. doi:10.​1016/​0028-3932(71)90067-4 CrossRef PubMed
    Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., & Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 156869. doi:10.​1155/​2011/​156869 CrossRef PubMed PubMedCentral
    Palmer, S. E. (1991). Goodness, Gestalt, groups, and Garner: Local symmetry subgroups as a theory of figural goodness. In G. R. Lockhead & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), The perception of structure (pp. 23–39). Washington: American Psychological Association.
    Perez-Abalo, M. C., Rodriguez, R., Bobes, M. A., Gutierrez, J., & Valdes-Sosa, M. (1994). Brain potentials and the availability of semantic and phonological codes over time. NeuroReport, 5, 2173–2177.CrossRef PubMed
    Peskin, J. (1998). Constructing meaning when reading poetry: An expert–novice study. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 235–263.CrossRef
    Port, R. F. (2003). Meter and speech. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 599–611.CrossRef
    Praamstra, P., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Neurophysiological manifestations of auditory phonological processing: Latency variation of a negative ERP component timelocked to phonological mismatch. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 204–219.CrossRef PubMed
    Praamstra, P., & Stegeman, D. F. (1993). Phonological effects on the auditory N400 event-related brain potential. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 73–86. doi:10.​1016/​0926-6410(93)90013-U CrossRef PubMed
    Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 364–382. doi:10.​1207/​s15327957pspr080​4_​3 CrossRef PubMed
    Roncaglia-Denissen, M. P., Schmidt-Kassow, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2013). Speech rhythm facilitates syntactic ambiguity resolution: ERP evidence. PLoS ONE, 8, e56000. doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0056000 CrossRef PubMed PubMedCentral
    Rothermich, K., Schmidt-Kassow, M., Schwartze, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2010). Event-related potential responses to metric violations: Rules versus meaning. NeuroReport, 21, 580–584.CrossRef PubMed
    Rothermich, K., Schmidt-Kassow, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2012). Rhythm’s gonna get you: Regular meter facilitates semantic sentence processing. Neuropsychologia, 50, 232–244. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuropsychologia​.​2011.​10.​025 CrossRef PubMed
    Rugg, M. D. (1984a). Event-related potentials and the phonological processing of words and non-words. Neuropsychologia, 22, 435–443.CrossRef PubMed
    Rugg, M. D. (1984b). Event-related potentials in phonological matching tasks. Brain and Language, 23, 225–240.CrossRef PubMed
    Rugg, M. D., & Barrett, S. E. (1987). Event-related potentials and the interaction between orthographic and phonological information in a rhyme-judgment task. Brain and Language, 32, 336–361.CrossRef PubMed
    Salimpoor, V. N., & Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Neural interactions that give rise to musical pleasure. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 62–75.CrossRef
    Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (1996). Natural music intervals: Evidence from infant listeners. Psychological Science, 7, 272–277.CrossRef
    Schmidt-Kassow, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2009a). Attention and perceptual regularity in speech. NeuroReport, 20, 1643–1647. doi:10.​1097/​WNR.​0b013e328333b0c6​ CrossRef PubMed
    Schmidt-Kassow, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2009b). Event-related brain potentials suggest a late interaction of meter and syntax in the P600. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1693–1708. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2008.​21153 CrossRef PubMed
    Selkirk, E. (1986). On derived domains in sentence phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 371–405.CrossRef
    Steinbeis, N., & Koelsch, S. (2008). Shared neural resources between music and language indicate semantic processing of musical tension–resolution patterns. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1169–1178.CrossRef PubMed
    Steinbeis, N., Koelsch, S., & Sloboda, J. A. (2006). The role of harmonic expectancy violations in musical emotions: Evidence from subjective, physiological, and neural responses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1380–1393.CrossRef PubMed
    Stumpf, C. (1885). Musikpsychologie in England: Betrachtungen über die Herleitung der Musik aus der Sprache und aus dem thierischen Entwickelungsproceß, über Empirismus und Nativismus in der Musiktheorie. Vierteljahrsschrift für Musikwissenschaft, 1, 261–349.
    Ticini, L. F., & Omigie, D. (2013). Why do we like what we like? When information flow matters. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 731. doi:10.​3389/​fnhum.​2013.​00731 PubMed PubMedCentral
    Tsur, R. (2008). Toward a theory of cognitive poetics (2nd ed.). Brighton: Academic Press.
    Van Peer, W. (1990). The measurement of metre: Its cognitive and affective functions. Poetics, 19, 259–275.CrossRef
    Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1993). Illusions of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1235–1253. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​19.​6.​1235
    Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
    Ystad, S., Magne, M., Farner, S., Pallone, G., Aramaki, M., Besson, M., & Kronland-Martinet, R. (2007). Electrophysiological study of algorithmically processed metric/rhythmic variations in language and music. EURASIP Journal on Audio, Speech, and Music Processing, 2007, 030194.CrossRef
    Zeki, S. (1999). Art and the brain. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 76–96.
  • 作者单位:Christian Obermeier (1)
    Sonja A. Kotz (1) (2)
    Sarah Jessen (1)
    Tim Raettig (3)
    Martin von Koppenfels (4)
    Winfried Menninghaus (5)

    1. Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, P.O. Box 500 355, 04303, Leipzig, Germany
    2. Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
    3. Department of Psychology and Psychotherapy, Witten/Herdecke University, Witten, Germany
    4. Institute of Comparative Literature, Ludwig Maximilians University, Munich, Germany
    5. Department of Language and Literature, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
  • 刊物主题:Cognitive Psychology; Neurosciences;
  • 出版者:Springer US
  • ISSN:1531-135X
文摘
Rhetorical theory suggests that rhythmic and metrical features of language substantially contribute to persuading, moving, and pleasing an audience. A potential explanation of these effects is offered by “cognitive fluency theory,” which stipulates that recurring patterns (e.g., meter) enhance perceptual fluency and can lead to greater aesthetic appreciation. In this article, we explore these two assertions by investigating the effects of meter and rhyme in the reception of poetry by means of event-related brain potentials (ERPs). Participants listened to four versions of lyrical stanzas that varied in terms of meter and rhyme, and rated the stanzas for rhythmicity and aesthetic liking. The behavioral and ERP results were in accord with enhanced liking and rhythmicity ratings for metered and rhyming stanzas. The metered and rhyming stanzas elicited smaller N400/P600 ERP responses than their nonmetered, nonrhyming, or nonmetered and nonrhyming counterparts. In addition, the N400 and P600 effects for the lyrical stanzas correlated with aesthetic liking effects (metered–nonmetered), implying that modulation of the N400 and P600 has a direct bearing on the aesthetic appreciation of lyrical stanzas. We suggest that these effects are indicative of perceptual-fluency-enhanced aesthetic liking, as postulated by cognitive fluency theory.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700