Mental rotation training: transfer and maintenance effects on spatial abilities
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Chiara Meneghetti ; Erika Borella ; Francesca Pazzaglia
  • 刊名:Psychological Research
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:January 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:80
  • 期:1
  • 页码:113-127
  • 全文大小:945 KB
  • 参考文献:Allen, G. L., Kirasic, K. C., Dobson, S. H., Long, R. G., & Beck, S. (1996). Predicting environmental learning from spatial abilities: an indirect route. Intelligence, 22, 327–355. doi:10.​1016/​S0160-2896(96)90026-4 .CrossRef
    Borella, E., Carretti, B., Cantarella, A., Riboldi, F., Zavagnin, M., & De Beni, R. (2013). Benefits of training visuospatial working memory in young-old and old-old. Developmental Psychology,. doi:10.​1037/​a0033818 .
    Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., Hutchison, S., Perrig-Chiello, P., Däpp, C., Müller, M., et al. (2008). Impact of working memory training on memory performance in old-old adults. Psychology and Aging, 23, 743–753. doi:10.​1037/​a0014342 .PubMed CrossRef
    De Lisi, R., & Cammarano, D. M. (1996). Computer experience and gender differences in undergraduate mental rotation performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 12, 351–361. doi:10.​1016/​0747-5632(96)00013-1 .CrossRef
    Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, D. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princenton: Educational Testing Service.
    Geiser, C., Lehmann, W., & Eid, M. (2006). Separating “rotators” from “nonrotators” in the mental rotation test: a multigroup latent class analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 261–293. doi:10.​1207/​s15327906mbr4103​_​2 .PubMed CrossRef
    Guilford, J. P., & Zimmerman, W. S. (1947). The Guilford-Zimmerman aptitude survey. Orange: Sheridan Psychological Services.
    Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial abilities at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34, 151–176. doi:10.​1016/​j.​intell.​2005.​09.​005 .CrossRef
    Hegarty, M., & Waller, D. (2004). A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities. Intelligence, 32, 175–191. doi:10.​1016/​j.​intell.​2003.​12.​001 .CrossRef
    Hegarty, M., & Waller, D. (2006). Individual differences in spatial abilities. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), Handbook of visuospatial thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    Heil, M., Rösler, F., Link, M., & Bajric, J. (1998). What is improved if a mental rotation task is repeated: the efficiency of memory access, or the speed of a transformation routine? Psychological Research, 61, 99–106. doi:10.​1007/​s004260050016 .PubMed CrossRef
    Jansen, P., Wiedenbauer, G., & Hahn, N. (2010). Manual rotation training improves direction estimations in a virtual environmental space. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 6–17. doi:10.​1080/​0954144080267848​7 .CrossRef
    Kozhevnikov, M., & Hegarty, M. (2001). A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability. Memory & Cognition, 29, 745–756. doi:10.​3758/​BF03200477 .CrossRef
    Laski, E. V., Casey, B., Yu, Q., Dulaney, A., Heyman, M., & Dearing, E. (2013). Spatial skills as a predictor of first-grade girls’ use of higher level arithmetic strategies. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 123–130. doi:10.​1016/​j.​lindif.​2012.​08.​001 .CrossRef
    Leone, G., Taine, M. C., & Droulez, J. (1993). The influence of long-term practice on mental rotation of 3-D objects. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 241–255. doi:10.​1016/​0926-6410(93)90008-S .PubMed CrossRef
    Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498. doi:10.​2307/​1130467 .PubMed CrossRef
    Lohman, D. F. (1988). Spatial abilities as traits, processes and knowledge. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 181–248). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
    Moreau, D. (2012). Differentiating two- from three-dimensional mental rotation training effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. iFirst,. doi:10.​1080/​17470218.​2012.​744761 .
    Moreau, D., Clerc, J., Mansy-Dannay, A., & Guerrien, A. (2012). Enhancing spatial ability through sport practice: Evidence for an effect of motor training on mental rotation performance. Journal of Individual Differences, 33, 83–88. doi:10.​1027/​1614-0001 .CrossRef
    Pazzaglia, F., & Meneghetti, C. (2012). Spatial text processing in relation to spatial abilities and spatial styles. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 972–980. doi:10.​1080/​20445911.​2012.​725716 .CrossRef
    Sartori, G., Job, R., & Tressoldi, P. E. (1995). Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva [Developmental disortographic and dislexia assessment battery]. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.
    Shepard, R. N., & Feng, C. A. (1972). A chronometric study of mental paper folding. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 228–243. doi:10.​1016/​0010-0285(72)90005-9 .CrossRef
    Sims, V. K., & Mayer, R. E. (2002). Domain specificity of spatial expertise: the case of video game players. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 97–115. doi:10.​1002/​acp.​759 .CrossRef
    Stransky, D., Wilcox, L. M., & Dubrowski, A. (2010). Mental rotation: cross-task training and generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 349–360. doi:10.​1037/​a0021702 .
    Terlecki, M. S., Newcombe, N. S., & Little, M. (2008). Durable and generalized effects of spatial experience on mental rotation: gender differences in growth patterns. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 996–1013. doi:10.​1002/​acp.​1420 .CrossRef
    Thurstone, L. L., & Jeffrey, T. E. (1956). Flags: A test of space thinking. Chicago: Industrial Relations Centre.
    Thurstone, T. G., & Thurstone, L. L. (1963). Primary mental ability. Chicago: Science Research Associates.
    Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013a). The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 352–402. doi:10.​1037/​a0028446 .PubMed CrossRef
    Uttal, D. H., Miller, D. I., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013b). Exploring and enhancing spatial thinking: links to achievement in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 367–373. doi:10.​1177/​0963721413484756​ .CrossRef
    Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of 3-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599–604. doi:10.​2466/​pms.​1978.​47.​2.​599 .PubMed CrossRef
    Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 817–835. doi:10.​1037/​a0016127 .CrossRef
    Wiedenbauer, G., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Manual training of mental rotation in children. Learning and Instruction, 18, 30–41. doi:10.​1016/​j.​learninstruc.​2006.​09.​009 .CrossRef
    Wright, R., Thompson, W. L., Ganis, G., Newcombe, N. S., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2008). Training generalized spatial skills. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 763–771. doi:10.​3758/​PBR.​15.​4.​763 .CrossRef
  • 作者单位:Chiara Meneghetti (1)
    Erika Borella (1)
    Francesca Pazzaglia (1)

    1. Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia, 8, 35131, Padua, Italy
  • 刊物类别:Behavioral Science
  • 刊物主题:Psychology
    Psychology
  • 出版者:Springer Berlin / Heidelberg
  • ISSN:1430-2772
文摘
One of the aims of research in spatial cognition is to examine whether spatial skills can be enhanced. The goal of the present study was thus to assess the benefit and maintenance effects of mental rotation training in young adults. Forty-eight females took part in the study: 16 were randomly assigned to receive the mental rotation training (based on comparing pairs of 2D or 3D objects and rotation games), 16 served as active controls (performing parallel non-spatial activities), and 16 as passive controls. Transfer effects to both untrained spatial tasks (testing both object rotation and perspective taking) and visual and verbal tasks were examined. Across the training sessions, the group given mental rotation training revealed benefits in the time it took to make judgments when comparing 3D and 2D objects, but their mental rotation speed did not improve. When compared with the other groups, the mental rotation training group did show transfer effects, however, in tasks other than those practiced (i.e., in object rotation and perspective-taking tasks), and these benefits persisted after 1 month. The training had no effect on visual or verbal tasks. These findings are discussed from the spatial cognition standpoint and with reference to the (rotation) training literature.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700