Is attention enough? A re-examination of the impact of feature-specific attention allocation on semantic priming effects in the pronunciation task
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Manuel Becker ; Karl Christoph Klauer…
  • 关键词:Evaluative priming ; Semantic priming ; Pronunciation task ; Replicability ; Task switching
  • 刊名:Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:February 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:78
  • 期:2
  • 页码:396-402
  • 全文大小:382 KB
  • 参考文献:Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 104–128. doi:10.​1006/​jesp.​1996.​0005 CrossRef
    De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2002). Affective priming of semantic categorization responses. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 643–666. doi:10.​1080/​0269993014300041​9 CrossRef
    De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., & Spruyt, A. (2001). Affective priming of pronunciation responses: Effects of target degradation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 85–91. doi:10.​1006/​jesp.​2000.​1437 CrossRef
    Everaert, T., Spruyt, A., & de Houwer, J. (2011). On the (u)nconditionality of automatic attitude activation: The valence proportion effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 125–132. doi:10.​1037/​a0022316 CrossRef PubMed
    Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238. doi:10.​1037/​0022-3514.​50.​2.​229 CrossRef PubMed
    Gast, A., Werner, B., Heitmann, C., Spruyt, A., & Rothermund, K. (2014). Evaluative stimulus (in)congruency impacts performance in an unrelated task. Experimental Psychology, 61, 187–195. doi:10.​1027/​1618-3169/​a000238 CrossRef PubMed
    Hermans, D., Van den Broeck, A., & Eelen, P. (1998). Affective priming using a color- naming task: A test of an affective-motivational account of affective priming effects. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 45, 136–148.PubMed
    Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 785–813. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196544 CrossRef
    Kahneman, D. (2003). Experiences of collaborative research. American Psychologist, 58, 723–730. doi:10.​1037/​0003-066X.​58.​9.​723 CrossRef PubMed
    Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 464–489. doi:10.​1037/​a0019561 CrossRef
    Klauer, K. C., Becker, M., & Spruyt, A. (2016). Evaluative priming in the pronunciation task: A preregistered replication and extension. Experimental Psychology (in press)
    Klauer, K. C., & Mierke, J. (2005). Task-set inertia, attitude accessibility, and compatibility-order effects: New evidence for a task-set switching account of the implicit association test effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 208–217. doi:10.​1177/​0146167204271416​ CrossRef PubMed
    Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2001). Does sunshine prime loyal? affective priming in the naming task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 727–751. doi:10.​1080/​713755986 CrossRef PubMed
    Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: Findings and theories. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 7–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Klauer, K. C., Roßnagel, C., & Musch, J. (1997). List-context effects in evaluative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 246–255. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​23.​1.​246 PubMed
    Klauer, K. C., Schmitz, F., Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Voss, A. (2010). Understanding the role of executive control in the implicit association test: Why flexible people have small IAT effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 595–619. doi:10.​1080/​1747021090307682​6 CrossRef
    Klein, O., Doyen, S., Leys, C., Magalhães de Saldanha da Gama, P. A., Miller, S., Questienne, L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Low hopes, high expectations: Expectancy effects and the replicability of behavioral experiments. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 572–584. doi:10.​1177/​1745691612463704​ CrossRef PubMed
    Klinger, M. R., Burton, P. C., & Pitts, G. S. (2000). Mechanisms of unconscious priming: I. Response competition, not spreading activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 441–455. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​26.​2.​441 PubMed
    Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 618–630. doi:10.​3758/​BF03212999 CrossRef
    Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy – psychophysics software in Python. Neuroscience Methods, 162, 8–13. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jneumeth.​2006.​11.​017 CrossRef
    Rothermund, K., & Werner, B. (2014). Affective priming in the valent/ neutral categorization task is due to affective matching, not encoding facilitation. Reply to Spruyt. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 570–576. doi:10.​1080/​02699931.​2013.​856778 CrossRef PubMed
    Simonsohn, U. (2015). Small telescopes: Detectability and the evaluation of replication results. Psychological Science, 26, 559–569.CrossRef PubMed
    Spruyt, A. (2014). Attention please: evaluative priming effects in a valent/ non-valent categorization task. Reply to Werner and Rothermund (2013). Cognition and Emotion, 28, 560–569. doi:10.​1080/​02699931.​2013.​832653 CrossRef PubMed
    Spruyt, A. & Tibboel, H. (2015). On the automaticity of the evaluative priming effect in the valent/non-valent categorization task. PLoS ONE 10: e0121564. doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0121564
    Spruyt, A., Clarysse, J., Vansteenwegen, D., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. (2010). Affect 4.0: A free software package for implementing psychological and psychophysiological experiments. Experimental Psychology, 57, 36–45. doi:10.​1027/​1618-3169/​a000005 CrossRef PubMed
    Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Evereart, T., & Hermans, D. (2012). Unconscious semantic activation depends on feature-specific attention allocation. Cognition, 122, 91–95. doi:10.​1016/​j.​cognition.​2011.​08.​017 CrossRef PubMed
    Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., & Eelen, P. (2007). Affective priming of nonaffective semantic categorization responses. Experimental Psychology, 54, 44–53. doi:10.​1027/​1618-3169.​54.​1.​44 CrossRef PubMed
    Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D. (2009). Modulation of automatic semantic priming by feature-specific attention allocation. Journal of Memory & Language, 61, 37–54. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jml.​2009.​03.​004 CrossRef
    Spruyt, A., Gast, A., & Moors, A. (2011). The sequential priming paradigm. A primer. In K. C. Klauer, C. Stahl, & A. Voss (Eds.), Cognitive methods in social psychology (pp. 48–77). New York: Guilford.
    Spruyt, A., & Hermans, D. (2008). Affective priming of naming responses does not depend on stimulus repetition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 237–241. doi:10.​1037/​1196-1961.​62.​4.​237 CrossRef PubMed
    Spruyt, A., Hermans, D., Pandelaere, M., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2004). On the replicability of the affective priming effect in the pronunciation task. Experimental Psychology (formerly Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie), 51, 109–115. doi:10.​1027/​1618-3169.​51.​2.​109 CrossRef
    Storbeck, J., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Preferences and inferences in encoding visual objects: A systematic comparison of semantic and affective priming. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 81–93. doi:10.​1177/​0146167203258855​ CrossRef PubMed
    Tressoldi, P. E. (2012). Replication unreliability in psychology: Elusive phenomena or “elusive” statistical power? Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 218. doi:10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2012.​00218 PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
    Wentura, D. (1999). Activation and inhibition of affective information: Evidence for negative priming in the evaluation task. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 65–91. doi:10.​1080/​026999399379375
    Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (2003). The “meddling-in” of affective information: A general model of automatic evaluation effects. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 51–86). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Werner, B., & Rothermund, K. (2013). Attention please: No affective priming effects in a valent/neutral-categorization task. Cognition & Emotion, 27,(1) 119–132. doi:10.​1080/​02699931.​2012.​711744
    Williams, J. N. (1996). Is automatic priming semantic? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 113–161. doi:10.​1080/​095414496383121 CrossRef
    Wolfe, J. M. (2013). Registered reports and replications in attention, perception, & psychophysics. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 781–783. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-013-0502-5 CrossRef
  • 作者单位:Manuel Becker (1)
    Karl Christoph Klauer (1)
    Adriaan Spruyt (2)

    1. Institut für Psychologie, Albert-Ludwigs-Universität Freiburg, D-79085, Freiburg, Germany
    2. Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 刊物主题:Cognitive Psychology;
  • 出版者:Springer US
  • ISSN:1943-393X
文摘
In a series of articles, Spruyt and colleagues have developed the Feature-Specific Attention Allocation framework, stating that the semantic analysis of task-irrelevant stimuli is critically dependent upon dimension-specific attention allocation. In an adversarial collaboration, we replicate one experiment supporting this theory (Spruyt, de Houwer, & Hermans, 2009; Exp. 3), in which semantic priming effects in the pronunciation task were found to be restricted to stimulus dimensions that were task-relevant on induction trials. Two pilot studies showed the capability of our laboratory to detect priming effects in the pronunciation task, but also suggested that the original effect may be difficult to replicate. In this study, we tried to replicate the original experiment while ensuring adequate statistical power. Results show little evidence for dimension-specific priming effects. The present results provide further insight into the malleability of early semantic encoding processes, but also show the need for further research on this topic. Keywords Evaluative priming Semantic priming Pronunciation task Replicability Task switching

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700