Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Lavanya Krishnan (1)
    Albert Kang (1)
    George Sperling (1)
    Ramesh Srinivasan (1)
  • 关键词:Spatial attention ; EEG ; SSVEP ; Video games ; Brain networks
  • 刊名:Brain Topography
  • 出版年:2013
  • 出版时间:January 2013
  • 年:2013
  • 卷:26
  • 期:1
  • 页码:83-97
  • 全文大小:769KB
  • 参考文献:1. Andersson CA, Bro R (2000) The N-way toolbox for MATLAB. Chemom Intell Lab Syst 52(1):1- CrossRef
    2. Awh E, Matsukura M, Serences JT (2003) Top-down control over biased competition during covert spatial orienting. J Exp Psychol Human 29(1):52-3
    3. Berry AS, Zanto TP, Rutman AM, Clapp WC, Gazzaley A (2009) Practice-related improvement in working memory is modulated by changes in processing external interference. J Neurophysiol 102(3):1779-789 CrossRef
    4. Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10(4):433-36 CrossRef
    5. Bro R (1996) Multiway calibration. Multilinear PLS. J Chemom 10(1):47-1 CrossRef
    6. Carrasco M, Penpeci-Talgar C, Eckstein M (2000) Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement. Vis Res 40(10-2):1203-215
    7. Carrasco M, Ling S, Read S (2004) Attention alters appearance. Nat Neurosci 7(3):308-13 CrossRef
    8. Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58(3):306-24 CrossRef
    9. Ding J, Sperling G, Srinivasan R (2006) Attentional modulation of SSVEP power depends on the network tagged by the flicker frequency. Cereb Cortex 16(7):1016-029 CrossRef
    10. Dosher BA, Lu ZL (1998) Perceptual learning reflects external noise filtering and internal noise reduction through channel reweighting. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95(23):13988-3993
    11. Dosher BA, Lu ZL (2000) Noise exclusion in spatial attention. Psychol Sci: A J Am Psychol Soc/APS 11(2):139-46 CrossRef
    12. Gobell JL, Tseng CH, Sperling G (2004) The spatial distribution of visual attention. Vis Res 44(12):1273-296 CrossRef
    13. Green CS, Bavelier D (2003) Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423(6939):534-37 CrossRef
    14. Green CS, Bavelier D (2006) Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 32(6):1465-478 CrossRef
    15. Green CS, Bavelier D (2007) Action-video-game experience alters the spatial resolution of vision. Psychol Sci: J Am Psychol Soc/APS 18(1):88-4 CrossRef
    16. Hillyard SA, Vogel EK, Luck SJ (1998) Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 353(1373):1257-270 CrossRef
    17. Kioumourtzoglou E, Kourtessis T, Michalopoulou M, Derri V (1998) Differences in several perceptual abilities between experts and novices in basketball, volleyball, and water-polo. Percept Mot Skills 86(3 Pt 1):899-12 CrossRef
    18. Luck SJ, Hillyard SA (1995) The role of attention in feature detection and conjunction discrimination: an electrophysiological analysis. Int J Neurosci 80(1-):281-97
    19. Mishra J, Zinni M, Bavelier D, Hillyard SA (2011) Neural basis of superior performance of action videogame players in an attention-demanding task. J Neurosci: Off J Soc Neurosci 31(3):992-98 CrossRef
    20. Morgan ST, Hansen JC, Hillyard SA (1996) Selective attention to stimulus location modulates the steady-state visual evoked potential. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93(10):4770-774 CrossRef
    21. Muller MM, Picton TW, Valdes-Sosa P, Riera J, Teder-Salejarvi WA, Hillyard SA (1998) Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential in the 20-28?Hz range. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 6(4):249-61 CrossRef
    22. Nougier V, Azemar G, Stein JF, Ripoll H (1992) Covert orienting to central visual cues and sport practice relations in the development of visual attention. J Exp Child Psychol 54(3):315-33 CrossRef
    23. Page EB (1963) Ordered hypotheses for multiple treatments: a significance test for linear ranks. J Am Stat Assoc 58(301):216-30
    24. Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10(4):437-42 CrossRef
    25. Serences JT, Yantis S (2006) Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence. Trends Cogn Sci 10(1):38-5 CrossRef
    26. Serences JT, Yantis S, Culberson A, Awh E (2004) Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor suppression during covert spatial orienting. J Neurophysiol 92(6):3538-545 CrossRef
    27. Srinivasan R (1999) Spatial structure of the human alpha rhythm: global correlation in adults and local correlation in children. Clin Neurophysiol: Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol 110(8):1351-362 CrossRef
    28. Srinivasan R, Bibi FA, Nunez PL (2006) Steady-state visual evoked potentials: distributed local sources and wave-like dynamics are sensitive to flicker frequency. Brain Topogr 18(3):167-87 CrossRef
    29. Westerhuis JA, de Jong S, Smilde AK (2001) Direct orthogonal signal correction. Chemom Intell Lab Syst 56(1):13-5 CrossRef
  • 作者单位:Lavanya Krishnan (1)
    Albert Kang (1)
    George Sperling (1)
    Ramesh Srinivasan (1)

    1. Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA, 92617, USA
  • ISSN:1573-6792
文摘
We investigated the psychophysical and neurophysiological differences between fast-action video game players (specifically first person shooter players, FPS) and non-action players (role-playing game players, RPG) in a visual search task. We measured both successful detections (hit rates) and steady-state visually evoked EEG potentials (SSVEPs). Search difficulty was varied along two dimensions: number of adjacent attended and ignored regions (1, 2 and 4), and presentation rate of novel search arrays (3, 8.6 and 20?Hz). Hit rates decreased with increasing presentation rates and number of regions, with the FPS players performing on average better than the RPG players. The largest differences in hit rate, between groups, occurred when four regions were simultaneously attended. We computed signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of SSVEPs and used partial least squares regression to model hit rates, SNRs and their relationship at 3?Hz and 8.6?Hz. The following are the most significant results: RPG players-parietal responses to the attended 8.6?Hz flicker were predictive of hit rate and were positively correlated with it, indicating attentional signal enhancement. FPS players-parietal responses to the ignored 3?Hz flicker were predictive of hit rate and were positively correlated with it, indicating distractor suppression. Consistent with these parietal responses, RPG players-frontal responses to the attended 8.6?Hz flicker, increased as task difficulty increased with number of regions; FPS players-frontal responses to the ignored 3?Hz flicker increased with number of regions. Thus the FPS players appear to employ an active suppression mechanism to deploy selective attention simultaneously to multiple interleaved regions, while RPG primarily use signal enhancement. These results suggest that fast-action gaming can affect neural strategies and the corresponding networks underlying attention, presumably by training mechanisms of distractor suppression.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700