理解双人肢体运动表达的友好和敌对意图的ERP证据
详细信息    查看全文 | 推荐本文 |
  • 英文篇名:Brain spatio-temporal dynamics of understanding kind versus hostile intentions based on dyadic body movements
  • 作者:黄亮 ; 杨雪 ; 黄志华 ; 王益文
  • 英文作者:HUANG Liang;YANG Xue;HUANG Zhihua;WANG Yiwen;Institute of Applied Psychology, Minnan Normal University;Institute of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Fuzhou University;Center for China Social Trust Research, Fuzhou University;College of Mathematics and Computer Science, Fuzhou University;
  • 关键词:心理理论 ; 友好意图 ; 敌对意图 ; 双人互动 ; 事件相关电位
  • 英文关键词:Theory of Mind;;kind intention;;hostile intention;;dyadic interaction;;ERP
  • 中文刊名:XLXB
  • 英文刊名:Acta Psychologica Sinica
  • 机构:闽南师范大学应用心理研究所;福州大学心理与认知科学研究所;福州大学中国社会信任研究中心;福州大学数学与计算机科学学院;
  • 出版日期:2019-03-21 11:21
  • 出版单位:心理学报
  • 年:2019
  • 期:v.51
  • 基金:国家自然科学基金面上项目(31771238)研究成果之一;; 福建省闽江学者支持计划;; 福建省社会科学规划青年项目(FJ2018C030);; 福建省自然科学基金项目(2019J01753);; 2017年福建省高校杰出青年科研人才培育计划;; 漳州市2018年度中青年领军人才访学进修计划资助
  • 语种:中文;
  • 页:XLXB201905003
  • 页数:14
  • CN:05
  • ISSN:11-1911/B
  • 分类号:35-48
摘要
已有的意图理解神经成像研究大多关注理解单一个体的中性或负性意图的脑功能定位,而大脑理解依靠双人肢体运动表达的友好和敌对意图的动态时间过程尚不清楚。本研究记录了20名健康被试完成三种不同意图推理任务时的脑电成分。三种意图分别为:1)友好意图; 2)敌对意图; 3)中性无互动意图。行为结果发现理解敌对意图的反应时最短。电生理学结果表明,在额中区的N250 (170~270 ms)上,中性意图比友好和敌对意图均更负,且友好意图比敌对意图也更负;在大脑右半球的P300(270~450ms)上,敌对意图比友好和中性意图均更正,且友好意图比中性意图也更正。对友好和敌对意图的N250和P300进行溯源分析分别定位于额中回(BA10)和脑岛(BA45)。结果表明大脑在多个阶段对双人肢体运动表达的互动意图进行分类理解,对负性敌对意图表现出较早的理解且晚期持续性的评价加工。
        Previous social neuroscience studies focused mainly on the neural networks that sustain an understanding of a single individual's neutral or negative intentions. However, few studies have explored those of different types of social interactive intentions performed by two or a number of individuals and their whole body movements. In addition, the spatio-temporal dynamics of understanding the kind and hostile intentions in the human brain also remain unclear.In order to address the above issue, the electroencephalograph(EEG) technique was employed to explore the dissociative neural correlates of understanding kind versus hostile intention. Twenty healthy participants were recruited for the experiment. Their behavioral data(accuracy and reaction time) and electrical brain activities were recorded while they were watching colorful photos depicting two actors' actions and performing an intention inference task(IIT). There were three different types of action intentions: kindness, hostility and non-interactiveness(neutrality). The ERP data was analyzed using the Scan and sLoreta software in an off-line way.The Univariate Analysis of Variance(ANOVA) with repeated measures of mean accuracy showed no significant difference among three conditions, while a main effect of condition existed for reaction time. The reaction times of hostile intention were shorter than those of the kind and neutral intentions, while no significant difference was found between the latter two conditions. The ERP data were analyzed using a three-way repeated measure ANOVA. The ANOVA factors were intention condition(kindness, hostility and neutrality), laterality(left,midline and right areas) and caudality(frontal, central and parietal areas). Electrophysiological results showed, over the frontal area, a significantly more negative amplitude of N250(170~270 ms) for neutral intention compared to kind and hostile intentions, and the N250 amplitudes for kind intention were also more negative than those for hostile intention, especially on the FZ electrode site. The source localization showed maximum activation in Broadman 10(BA10), in the vicinity of middle frontal gyrus, for N250 for kind and hostile intentions. At the later stage(270~450 ms), the peak amplitudes of the P300 for hostile intention were more positive than those for the kind and neutral intentions, and the P300 amplitudes for kind intention were also more positive than those for neutral intention over the central, parietal areas as well as the right hemisphere. The maximum activation for P300 of kind and hostile intentions was found in BA45, located in the vicinity of insula, and a stronger activity existed for hostile intention compared to kind intention.These findings show that there is a spatio-temporal dynamic dissociation between kind and hostile intentions understanding in the brain. Altogether, the current study provides electrophysiological evidence underlying the kind, hostile interactive intentions and non-interactive(neutral) intention understanding, and suggests a prioritized and sustained processing for hostile interactive intention. The study enriches the contents of the two-stage intention-understanding model and the putative Hierarchical & Multi-level Cognitive Framework(HMCF) in Theory of Mind.
引文
Adenzato,M.,Brambilla,M.,Manenti,R.,de Lucia,L.,Trojano,L.,Garofalo,S.,....Cotelli,M.(2017).Gender differences in cognitive theory of mind revealed by transcranial direct current stimulation on medial prefrontal cortex.Scientific Reports,7,41219.
    Amoruso,L.,Finisguerra,A.,Urgesi,C.(2018).Contextualizing action observation in the predictive brain:Causal contributions of prefrontal and middle temporal areas.NeuroImage,177,68-78.
    Ansuini,C.,Cavallo,A.,Bertone,C.,&Becchio,C.(2015).Intentions in the brain:The unveiling of Mister Hyde.Neuroscientist,21(2),126-135.
    Bahnemann,M.,Dziobek,I.,Prehn,K.,Wolf,I.,&Heekeren,H.R.(2010).Sociotopy in the temporoparietal cortex:Common versus distinct processes.Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,5(1),48-58.
    Berntson,G.G.,Norman,G.J.,Bechara,A.,Bruss,J.,Tranel,D.,&Cacioppo,J.T.(2011).The insula and evaluative processes.Psychological Science,22(1),80-86.
    Blakemore,S.J.,&Decety,J.(2001).From the perception of action to the understanding of intention.Nature Review Neuroscience,2(8),561-567.
    Borhani,K.,Borgomaneri,S.,Làdavas,E.,&Bertini,C.(2016).The effect of alexithymia on early visual processing of emotional body postures.Biological Psychology,115,1-8.
    Buon,M.,Jacob,P.,Margules,S.,Brunet,I.,Dutat,M.,Cabrol,D.,&Dupoux,E.(2014).Friend or foe?Early social evaluation of human interactions.Plos ONE,9(2),e88612.
    Cacioppo,J.T.,Berntson,G.G.,&Decety,J.(2010).Social neuroscience and its relationship to social psychology.Social Cognition,28(6),675-685.
    Cacioppo,S.,Juan,E.,&Monteleone,G.(2017).Predicting intentions of a familiar significant other beyond the mirror neuron system.Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience,11,155.
    Carter,E.J.,&Pelphrey,K.A.(2008).Friend or foe?Brain systems involved in the perception of dynamic signals of menacing and friendly social approaches.Social Neuroscience,3(2),151-163.
    Catmur,C.(2015).Understanding intentions from actions:Direct perception,inference,and the roles of mirror and mentalizing systems.Consciousness and Cognition,36,426-433.
    Centelles,L.,Assaiante,C.,Nazarian,B.,Anton,J.-L.,&Schmitz,C.(2011).Recruitment of both the mirror and the mentalizing networks when observing the social interactions depicted by point-lights:A neuroimaging study.Plos ONE,6(1),e15749.
    Decety,J.&Cacioppo,S.(2012).The speed of morality:Ahigh-density electrical neuroimaging study.Journal of Neurophysiology,108(11),3068-3072.
    Gan,T.,Shi,R.,Liu,C.&Luo Y.J.(2018).Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation on the right temporo-parietal junction modulates the helpful intention processing.Acta Psychologica Sinica,50(1),36-46.[甘甜,石睿,刘超,罗跃嘉.(2018).经颅直流电刺激右侧颞顶联合区对助人意图加工的影响.心理学报,50(1),36-46.]
    Georgescu,A.L.,Kuzmanovic,B.,Santos,N.S.,Tepest,R.,Bente,G.,Tittgemeyer,M.,&Vogeley,K.(2014).Perceiving nonverbal behavior:Neural correlates of processing movement fluency and contingency in dyadic interactions.Human Brain Mapping,35(4),1362-1378.
    Gilead,M.,Katzir,M.,Eyal,T.,&Liberman,N.(2016).Neural correlates of processing"self-conscious"vs."basic"emotions.Neuropsychologia,81,207-218.
    Grafton,S.T.(2009).Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others.Annals New York Academy of Science,1156,97-117.
    Güro?lu,B.,Haselager,G.J.T.,van Liershout,C.F.M.,Takashima,A.,Rijpkema,M.,&Fernández,G.(2008)Why are friends special?Implementing a social interaction simulation task to probe the neural correlates of friendship.NeuroImage,39(2),903-910.
    Huang,L.,Deng,Z.X.,Ren,H.L.,Lin,G.Y.,&Wang Y.W.(2018).The role of stimulus ecologicalness and genders in understanding kind versus hostile intentions.Journal of Psychological Science,41(2),435-440.[黄亮,邓兆鑫,任翰林,林国耀,王益文.(2018).材料生态性和性别因素对理解友好与敌对意图的影响.心理科学,41(2),435-440.]
    Isoda,M.(2016).Understanding intentional actions from observers'viewpoints:A social neuroscience perspective.Neuroscience Research,112,1-9.
    Kaiser,M.D.,Pelphrey,K.A.(2012).Disrupted action perception in autism:Behavioral evidence,neuroendophenotypes,and diagnostic utility.Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,2(1),25-35.
    Liberman,Z.,Kinzler,K.D.,&Woodward,A.L.(2014).Friends or foes:Infants use shared evaluations to infer others'social relationships.Journal of Experimental Psychology:General,143(3),966-971.
    Liu,D.,Sabbagh,M.A.,Gehring,W.J.,&Wellman,H.M.(2004).Decoupling beliefs from reality in the brain:An ERP study of theory of mind.Neuroreport,15(6),991-995.
    Malle,B.F.,&Holbrook,J.(2012).Is there a hierarchy of social inferences?The likelihood and speed of inferring intentionality,mind,and personality.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,102(4),661-684.
    Maranesi,M.,Livi,A.,Fogassi,L.,Rizzolatti,G.,&Bonini,L.(2014)Mirror neuron activation prior to action observation in a predictable context.Journal of Neuroscience,34(45),14827-14832.
    Moser,J.S.,Huppert,J.D.,Duval,E.,&Simons,R.F.(2008).Face processing biases in social anxiety:An electrophysiological study.Biological Psychology,78(1),93-103.
    M?tt?nen,R.,Farmer,H.,&Watkins,K.E.(2016).Neural basis of understanding communicative actions:Changes associated with knowing the actor’s intention and the meanings of the actions.Neuropsychologia,81,230-237.
    Ortigue,S.,Sinigaglia,C.,Rizzolatti,G.,&Grafton,S.T.(2010).Understanding actions of others:The electrodynamics of the left and right hemispheres.A high-density EEGneuroimaging study.Plos One,5(8),e12160.
    Proverbio,A.M.,Riva,F.,Paganelli,L.,Cappa,S.F.,Canessa,N.,Perani,D.,&Zani,A.(2011).Neural coding of cooperative vs.affective human interactions:150 ms to code the action's purpose.Plos One,6(7),e22026.
    Sinke,C.B.A.,Sorger,B.,Goebel,R.,&de Gelder,B.(2010).Tease or threat?Judging social interactions from bodily expressions.NeuroImage,49(2),1717-1727.
    van Wount,M.,&Sanfey,A.G.(2008).Friend or foe:The effect of implicit trustworthiness judgments in social decision-making.Cognition,108(3),796-803.
    Vrticka,P.,Andersson,F.,Sander,D.,&Vuilleumier,P.(2009).Memory for friends or foes:The social context of past encounters with faces modulates their subsequent neural traces in the brain.Social Neuroscience,4(5),384-401.
    Walter,H.,Adenzato,M.,Ciaramidaro,A.,Enrici,I.,Pia,L.&Bara,B.G.(2004).Understanding intentions in social interaction:The role of the anterior paracingulate cortex.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,16(10),1854-1863.
    Wang,Y.W.,Huang,L.,Lin,C.,Zhang,Z.,Liang,F.,&Shen,D.(2012).Spatio-temporal brain dynamics of understanding social versus private intentions:An electrical neuroimaging study.Neuroquantology,10(4),733-743.
    Wang,Y.W.,Huang,L.,Xu,S.,Yuan,B.,Xu,Y.J.,&Li,H.Y.(2012).ERP time course of understanding private versus communicative intentions.Acta Psychologica Sinica,44(12),1618-1627.[王益文,黄亮,徐晟,袁博,徐艳娇,李洪玉.(2012).理解私人意图与交际意图的ERP证据.心理学报,44(12),1618-1627.]
    Wang,Y.W.,Huang,L.,Zhang,Z.,Song,J.,&Bai,L.Y.(2014).Kindness or hostility?Electrical brain dynamics of understanding interactive intentions of other people.Science in China,Series C,Life Science,44(7),736-746.[王益文,黄亮,张振,宋娟,白丽英.(2014).友好还是敌对?理解互动意图的大脑动态时程.中国科学:生命科学,44(7),736-746.]
    Wang,Y.W.,Huang,L.,Zhang,W.,Zhang,Z.,&Cacioppo,S.(2015).Spatio-temporal dynamics of kind versus hostile intentions in the human brain:An electrical neuroimaging study.Social Neuroscience,10(3),253-267.
    Wang,Y.W.,Lin,C.D.,Yuan,B.,Huang,L.,&Shen,D.L.(2010).Person perception precedes theory of mind:An event related potential analysis.Neuroscience,170(1),238-246.
    Wang,Y.W.,Zheng,Y.W.,Lin,C.D.,Wu,J.,&Shen,D.L.(2011).Electrophysiological correlates of reading the single-and interactive-mind.Frontier in Human Neuroscience,5,64.
    Wang,Y.W.,Zheng,Y.W.,Shen,D.L.,Cui,L.,&Yan,G.L.(2012).Reading another’s mind and reading the interactive mind from Chinese idioms:Evidience from eye-movements and ERPs.Acta Psychologica Sinica,44(1),100-111.[王益文,郑玉玮,沈德立,崔磊,闫国利.(2012).解读成语中的他人心理与互动心理:来自眼动和ERP的证据.心理学报,44(1),100-111.]
    Watanabe,T.,Yahata,N.,Abe,O.,Kuwabara,H.,Inoue,H.,Takano,Y.,...Yamasue,H.(2012).Diminished medial prefrontal activity behind autistic social judgments of incongruent information.Plos ONE,7(6),e39561.
    Weymar,M.,Bradley,M.M.,Hamm,A.O.,&Lang,P.J.(2013).When fear forms memories:Threat of shock and brain potentials during encodingand recognition.Cortex,49(3),819-826.
    Wieser,M.J.,McTeague,L.M.,&Keil,A.(2011).Sustained preferential processing of social threat cues:Bias without competition.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,23(8),1973-1986.
    Wu,H.,Chen,C.,Cheng,D.,Yang,S.,Huang,R.,Cacioppo,S.,&Luo,Y-J.(2014).The mediation effect of menstrual phase on negative emotion processing:Evidence from N2.Social Neuroscience,9(3),278-288.
    Yoder,K.J.,&Decety,J.(2014).The good,the bad,and the just:Justice sensitivity predicts neural response during moral evaluation of actions performed by others.Journal of Neuroscience,34(12),4161-4166.
    Young,L.,&Saxe,R.(2009).Innocent intentions:Acorrelation between forgiveness for accidental harm and neural activity.Neuropsychologia,47(10),2065-2072.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700