秦岭川金丝猴(Rhinopithecus roxellana)繁殖竞争与肢体偏好的研究
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
第一部分秦岭川金丝猴繁殖竞争的研究
     第一章秦岭川金丝猴主雄替换后单元内雌雄两性繁殖策略的研究
     2003年3月至2004年5月,利用焦点动物取样法和行为取样法对分布在秦岭北坡周至国家自然保护区玉皇庙地区川金丝猴西梁群关于主雄替换对单元内两性繁殖关系的影响进行研究。研究结果发现:主雄替换后,单元新主雄会倾向于自主选择非处于哺乳期成年雌性作为主要繁殖对象。新主雄通过单元外交配行为,不仅可以增加扩散自身基因的可能性,而且可以刺激单元内交配行为的发生进而增加自身繁殖成功机会。哺乳期雌性个体在其婴猴消失后的首个生育间隔显著短于研究群内未失婴猴雌性的平均生育间隔,这意味着野生成年雌性川金丝猴面对自身婴猴消失会采取积极的适应性繁殖策略。此外,亚成年雌性川金丝猴通过积极与新主雄建立良好繁殖关系进而获得其在成年后的繁殖成功。总之,野生川金丝猴单元新主雄、成年雌性和亚成年雌性个体面对主雄替换及之后的婴猴消失各自采取了不同的繁殖策略,这在一定程度上体现了该物种对于主雄替换这一避免近亲繁殖机制的行为适应性。
     第二章秦岭川金丝猴雌性扩散背后的交配竞争及相关繁殖行为研究
     2001年10月至2005年5月,对分布在秦岭北坡周至国家自然保护区玉皇庙地区川金丝猴西梁群关于雌性个体扩散与繁殖成功关系进行研究。研究结果发现:成年雌性个体和业成年雌性个体均发生扩散,成年雌性个体的迁出率和迁入率均高于亚成年雌性个体。交配竞争是成年雌性个体扩散的主要推动力,而亚成年雌性个体扩散可能是为了避免近亲繁殖。成年雌性个体在扩散过程中通过采用多样化的繁殖策略进而提高自身繁殖成功率。
     第三章秦岭川金丝猴单元大小及组成对雌性繁殖成功影响的研究
     动物的社会性在其群居利益大于群居代价时得以进化。群组大小和群组构成是评价动物社会性的两个基础衡量尺度,它们对雌性繁殖成功发挥重要影响作用。2001年10月至2008年5月,对分布在秦岭北坡周至国家自然保护区玉皇庙地区川金丝猴西梁群关于一雄多雌单元大小和组成对川金丝猴雌性繁殖成功的影响进行长期研究。研究结果发现:川金丝猴雌性繁殖成功与不同单元大小组别的平均值和与单元内成年雌性数目均呈显著负线性相关。川金丝猴的这一研究结果支持van Schaik(1983)的掠夺-组内取食竞争假说。组内扰乱竞争可能存在于川金丝猴社群中,并影响着雌性繁殖成功。
     第二部分秦岭川金丝猴肢体偏好的研究
     第四章秦岭川金丝猴单手和双手触达任务中手偏好行为的研究
     2003年7月至2004年1月,利用焦点动物取样法和行为取样法对分布在秦岭北坡周至国家自然保护区玉皇庙地区的川金丝猴西梁群成年个体的手偏好行为进行观察研究。在单手取食触达和双手爬背触达两个行为任务中,焦点动物个体的左手偏好和右手偏好均在不同程度上分别有所体现,且无性别因素影响。64.29%的焦点动物个体在单手取食触达任务中表现出明显的手偏好,100%的焦点动物个体在双手爬背触达任务中表现出明显的手偏好。根据手偏好指数,71.43%的焦点动物个体在单手取食触达任务中表现出左手偏好,这在一定程度上支持姿势起源理论。川金丝猴在双手爬背触达任务中的手偏好强度明显高于其在单手取食触达任务中的手偏好强度,这说明身体姿势是川金丝猴手偏好表达的重要影响因素之一。
     第五章秦岭川金丝猴四肢前行和双足前移任务中脚偏好行为的研究
     2007年3月至2008年4月,利用焦点动物行为取样法对分布在秦岭北坡周至国家自然保护区玉皇庙地区的川金丝猴西梁群成年个体在移动中的脚偏好行为进行观察研究。本研究选用了四肢前行和双足前移两个行为任务,研究中选择21个成年个体作为焦点动物,其中包括9个成年雄性和12个成年雌性。在两个行为任务中,焦点动物个体的左脚偏好和右脚偏好均在不同程度上分别有所体现,且无性别因素影响。57.14%的焦点动物个体在四肢前行任务中表现出明显的脚偏好,100%的焦点动物个体在双足前移任务中表现出明显的脚偏好。根据脚偏好指数,61.90%的焦点动物个体在四肢前行任务中表现出右脚偏好,这在一定程度上支持姿势起源理论。川金丝猴在双足前移任务中的脚偏好强度明显高于其在四肢前行任务中的脚偏好强度,这说明身体姿势是川金丝猴脚偏好表达的重要影响因素之一。
     第六章秦岭川金丝猴新生婴猴吸乳偏好和母猴携抱偏好行为的研究
     2007年3月至4月,利用焦点动物行为取样法对分布在秦岭北坡周至国家自然保护区玉皇庙地区的野生川金丝猴西梁群个体的早期母婴互动行为不对称性(即婴猴吸乳偏好和母猴携抱偏好)进行观察研究。本研究选择10个焦点母婴对,结果发现,母猴携抱偏好和婴猴吸乳偏好在个体水平上表达显著,在群组水平上表达不显著;母猴携抱偏好与婴猴吸乳偏好之间相关不显著:野生川金丝猴在出生后一周内即表现出明显的头部转向偏好。
Part One
     Reproductive Competition of Sichuan Snub-nosed Monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains,China
     Chapter 1:Impact of male takeover on intra-unit sexual interactions and subsequent inter-birth interval in Rhinopithecus roxellana
     Data were collected on sexual interactions before and after a male takeover of a one male unit(OMU) of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys(Rhinopithecus roxellana) at the Zhouzhi National Nature Reserve in the Qinling Mountains of China.The original unit consisted of an adult male,two adult and two subadult females,two female juveniles and a single infant.Following the takeover,the new resident male copulated with one adult female, which was not lactating.Subsequent to the disappearance of her infant,the second(lactating) female entered breeding condition and began to solicit copulation with the new resident male. Subadult females also engaged in matings with the new male.The new resident male was observed mating,on three occasions,with females in two other OMUs.These are the first observations of sexual behavior in free-ranging Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys after an OMU takeover.Sexual interactions play an important role in establishing relationships between the new male and resident females in the OMU.
     Chapter 2:Mate competition and reproductive correlates of female dispersal in Rhinopithecus roxellana
     Different mating systems in group-living animals have characteristic behavioral correlates that are primarily related to mate competition.Mate competition may push individuals to selectively make dispersal decisions for the purpose of maximizing of opportunities for reproduction.The Sichuan snub-nosed monkey(Rhinopithecus roxellana) is a polygynous primate species endemic to China.We provide the first data on female dispersal in a free-ranging group of R.roxellana at the Zhouzhi National Nature Reserve in the Qinling Mountains of China.Both adult and subadult female dispersal occurred. Immigration/emigration rates of adult females are higher than those of subadult females.Mate competition is one apparent driving force behind adult female dispersal,and inbreeding avoidance is the possible proximate factor influencing subadult female dispersal.Adult female R.roxellana employ various reproductive strategies related to dispersal,which may increase their reproductive success.
     Chapter 3:Effect of group size and composition on female reproductive success in Rhinopithecus roxellana
     Animal sociality evolves when gregarious benefits exceed those costs.Group size and group composition,as two basic dimensions of animal sociality,has various effects on the morphological and behavioral variables of group-living animals,such as individual reproductive success.This is the first study on the effect of group size and group composition on female reproductive success in the Rhinopithecus spp.From 2001 to 2008,our data was collected for a flee-ranging group of R.roxellana at the Zhouzhi National Nature Reserve in the Qinling Mountains of China.Female reproductive success of R.roxellana has a significantly linear correlation with both the mean size of each size group and the number of adult females.It suggests that the R.roxellana accords with the basic assumptions of the van Schaik(1983)'s predation-intragroup feeding competition hypothesis.This result raises the possibility that the within-group scramble competition works on female reproductive success in R.roxellana.
     Part Two
     Limb Laterality of Sichuan Snub-nosed Monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains,China
     Chapter 4:Hand preference during unimanual and bimanual reaching actions in Rhinopithecus roxellana
     Hand preferences were investigated during one unimanual action(food-reaching) and one bimanual action(mount-reaching) in a free-ranging group of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys(Rhinopithecus roxellana) at the Zhouzhi National Nature Reserve in the Qinling Mountains of China.Nine of fourteen individuals tested on the unimanual food-reaching action and all six individuals tested on the bimanual mount-reaching action exhibited a manual preference.Both significant right- and left-handed preferences were observed in the two actions.Sex did not affect either direction or strength of hand preference in the unimanual action.Hand preference for the bimanual action was stable over time,and the strength of hand preference was significantly stronger in the bimanual action than in the unimanual action.
     Chapter 5:Foot Preference during quadrupedal and bipedal actions in Rhinopithecus roxellana
     Foot preferences were investigated during the quadrupedal walking action and the bipedal shifting action for a free-ranging group of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) at the Zhouzhi National Nature Reserve in the Qinling Mountains of China.Twelve of 21 individuals tested on the quadrupedal action and all 21 individuals tested on the bipedal action exhibited a significant foot preference.Both significant right- and left-footed preferences were observed and sex did not affect either direction or strength of foot preference in both actions.The finding that 61.90%of focal R.roxellana showed the right-foot preference both in quadrupedal action based on the footed index and in bipedal action based on the z-score are in partial agreement with the postural origin hypothesis on footedness.The strength of foot preference was significantly stronger in the bipedal action than in the quadrupedal action,supporting the view that posture could be a crucial factor influencing foot preference as well as hand preference in this species.
     Chapter 6:Neonate nipple preference and maternal cradling laterality in Rhinopithecus roxellana
     This study of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys(Rhinopithecus roxellana) constitutes the first report on asymmetric patterns of early mother-infant interactions among Old World monkeys in the wild.It was found that neonate nipple preference and maternal cradling laterality are both evident on the individual level.Although there is no significant group-level preference direction,the group preference strength on both measures is evident.Moreover, neonate nipple preference is not significantly correlated with maternal cradling laterality on either LBI scores or the direction.Taken together,it is possible to suggest that wild R. roxellana showy independent orientation laterality from the first week of life.
引文
陈服官.1989.金丝猴研究进展.西安.西北大学出版社.
    陈服官,闵芝兰,罗时有,谢文治.1983.秦岭地区金丝猴的群体行为与生态习性的观察.兽类学报,3:141-146.
    李保国,李宏群,赵大鹏,张育辉.2006.秦岭川金丝猴一个投食群等级关系的研究.兽类学报,26:18-25.
    马原野,田云芬,邓紫云.1988.叶猴、金丝猴、长臂猿自由取食时的利手现象.人类学学报,7:177-181.
    全国强,谢家骅.2002.金丝猴研究.上海.上海科技教育出版社.
    任宝平,夏述忠,李庆芬,张树义,梁冰,邱军华.2002.圈养雄性川金丝猴交配模式.动物学报,48:577-584.
    任仁眉,严康慧,苏彦捷,周茵,李进军,朱兆泉,胡振林,胡云峰.2000.金丝猴的社会野外研究.北京.北京大学出版社.
    赵大鹏,2005.秦岭金丝猴(Rhinopithecus roxellana)交配行为的研究.硕士论文.西安.西北大学.
    赵大鹏,李保国.2005.川金丝猴射精标准的量化辅助.兽类学报,25:293-296.
    Agoramoorthy G.1994.Adult male replacement and social change in two troops of hanuman langurs(Presbytis entellus) at Jodhpur,India.International Journal of Primatology,15:225-238.
    Agoramoorthy G,Mohnot SM.1988.Infanticide and juvenilicide in hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) around Jodhpur,India.Human Evolution,3:279-296.
    Agoramoorthy G,Minna JH.2000.Extragroup copulation among wild red howler monkeys in Venezuela. Folia Primatologica, 71: 152-153.
    Altmann J. 1974. Observational study of behaviour: sampling method. Behaviour, 49: 227- 267.
    Altmann S, Wagner S, Levington S. 1977. Two models for the evolution of polygyny. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2: 397-410.
    Annett M. 2002. Handedness and Brian Asymmetry: The Right Shift Theory. Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press.
    Barton RA. 1996. Neocortex size and behavioural ecology in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 263: 173-177.
    Barton RA, Dunbar RIM. 1997. Evolution of the social brain. In: Whiten A, Byrne RW. eds. Machiavellian Intelligence II: Extensions and Evaluations. Cambridge. Cambridge Unviersity Press. pp. 240-263.
    Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Whitten PL. 2005. The effect of new alpha males on female stress in free-ranging baboons. Animal Behaviour, 69: 1211-1221.
    
    Blois-Heulin C, Guitton JS, Nedellec-Bienvenue D, Ropars L, Vallet E. 2006. Hand preference in unimanual and bimanual tasks and postural effect on manual laterality in captive red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebits torquatus torqiiatus). American Journal of Primatology, 68: 429-444.
    
    Borries C. 1993. Ecology of female social relationships: hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) and the van Schaik model. Folia Primatologica, 61: 21-30.
    
    Borries C, Lamey E, Derby AM, Koenig A. 2004. Temporary absence and dispersal in Phayre's leaf monkeys (Trachypithecus phayrei). Folia Primatologica, 75: 27-30.
    
    Borries C, Larney E, Lu A, Ossi K, Koenig A. 2008. Cost of group size: lower developmental and reproductive rates in larger groups of leaf monkeys. Behavioral Ecology, 19:1186-1191.
    Bradshaw JL, Rogers LJ. 1993. The Evolution of Lateral Asymmetries, Language, Tool use, and Intellect. New York. Academic Press.
    Brockelman WY, Reichard U, Treesucon U, Raemaekers JJ. 1998. Dispersal, pair formation and social structure in gibbons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 42: 329-339.
    Brothers L. 2002. The social brain: A project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. In: Cacioppo JT, Berntson GG, Adolphs R, Carter CS, Davidson RJ, McClintock MK, McEwen BS, Meaney MJ, Schacter DL, Sternberg EM, Suomi SS, Taylor SE, eds. Foundations in Social Neuroscience. Cambridge. MIT Press. pp. 367-385.
    Brown ER, Taylor P. 1988. Handedness, footedness, and eyedness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66: 183-186.
    Butynski TM. 1982. Harem male replacement and infanticide in the blue monkey (Cercopithecus mitus stuhlmanni) in the Kibale Forest, Uganda. American Journal of Primatology, 3: 1-22.
    Calvin WH. 1983. A stone's throw and its launch window: timing precision and its implications for language and hominid brains. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 104: 121-135.
    Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, Mackinnon KC, Panger M, Bearder SK. 2007. Primates in Perspective. Oxford. Oxford University Press.
    Carey DP, Smith DT, Martin D, Smith G, Skriver J, Rutland A, Shepherd JW. 2009. The bipedal ape: plasticity and asymmetry in footedness. Cortex, 45: 650-661.
    Chapman JP, Chapman LJ, Allen JJ. 1987. The measurement of foot preference. Neuropsychologia, 25: 579-584.
    Chapman CA, Chapman LJ. 2000. Constraints on group size in red colobus and red-tailed guenons: examining the generality of the ecological constraints models. International Journal of Primatology, 21: 565-585.
    Chapman CA. Pavelka MSM. 2005. Group size in folivorous primates: Ecological constraints the possible influence of social factors. Primates, 64: 1-9.
    Clark DA, Mitra PP, Wang SSH. 2001. Scalable architecture in mammalian brains. Nature, 411: 189-193.
    Clarke MR, Zucker E, Glander KE.1994. Group takeover by a natal male howling monkey (Alouatta palliata) and associated disappearance and injuries of immatures. Primates, 35: 435-442.
    
    Clobert J, Danchin E, Dohndt AA, Nichols JD. 2001. Dispersal. Oxford. Oxford University- Press.
    Clouse R. 2001. Some effects of group size on the output of beginning nests of Mischocyttarus mexicanus (Hymenoptera: vespidae). Florida Entomologist, 84: 418-422.
    Clutton-Brock TH, Russell AF, Sharpe LL, Brotherton PNM, Mcilrath GM, White S, Cameon EZ. 2001.Effects of helpers on juvenile development and survival in meerkats. Science, 293: 2446-2449
    Corballis MC. 1991. The Lopsided Ape: Evolution of the Generative Mind. New York. Oxford University Press.
    Corballis MC. 1997. The genetics and evolution of handedness. Psychological Review, 104: 714-727.
    Corballis MC. 2002. From Hand to Mouth: The Origin of Language. Princeton. Princeton University Press.
    Coren S. 1993. The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: Norms for young adults. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 31:1-3.
    Cunningham D, Forsythe C, Ward JP. 1989. A report of behavioral lateralization in an infant orangutan (Pongopygmaeus). Primates, 30: 249-253. Cunningham EJA, Birkhead TR. 1998. Sex roles and sexual selection. Animal Behaviour, 56: 1311-1321.
    Damerose E, Hopkins WD. 2002. Scan and focal sampling: reliability in the lateraliy for maternal cradling, and infant nipple preferences in Olive baboons, Papio anubis. Animal Behaviour, 63: 511-518.
    Damerose E, Vauclair J. 2002. Posture and laterality in human and non-human primates: asymmetries in maternal handling and the infant's early motor asymmetries. In: Andrew RJ. ed. Comparative Vertebrate Lateralization. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 306-362.
    Dammhahn M, Kappeler PM. 2009. Females go where the food is: does the socio-ecological model explain variation in social organization of solitary foragers? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63: 939-952.
    Davies NB. 1991. Mating systems. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB. eds. Behavioural Ecology. Oxford. Blackwell. pp. 263-294.
    Day LB, MacNeilage PF. 1996. Postural asymmetries and language lateralization in humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110: 88-96.
    Deets AC, Harlow HF. 1970. Nipple preference in nursing singleton- and twin-reared rhesus monkey infant. Developmental Psychology, 2: 159-162.
    Dienske H, Hopkins B, Reid AK. 1995. Lateralisation of infant holding in chimpanzees: new data do not confirm previous findings. Behaviour, 132: 9-10.
    Dixson AF. 1998. Primate Sexuality. Oxford. Oxford University Press.
    Dunbar RIM. 1988. Primate Social Systems. New York. Cornell University Press.
    Dunbar RIM. 1998. The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178-190.
    Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. 2007. Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317: 1344-1347.
    Elias LJ, Bryden MP, Bulman FMB. 1998. Footedness is a better predictor than is handedness of emotional lateralization. Neuropsychologia, 36: 37-43.
    Enstam KL, Isbell LA, de Maar TW. 2002. Male demography, female mating behavior and infanticide in wild patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas). International Journal of Primatology, 23: 85-104.
    Erwin J, Anderson B, Bunger D. 1975. Nursing behavior of infant pigtail monkeys (Macaca nemestrina): preference for nipples. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54: 592-594.
    Fagot J, Vauclair J. 1991. Manual laterality in nonhuman primates: a distinction between handedness and manual specialization. Psychological Bulletin, 109: 76-89.
    Forsythe C, Ward JP. 1987. The lateralized behavior of the raffled lemur (Varecia variegata). American Journal of Primatology, 12: 342.
    Frost GT. 1980. Tool behavior and the origins of laterality. Journal of Human Evolution, 9: 447-459.
    Gentry V, Gabbard C. 1995. Foot preference behavior: a. developmental perspective. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 122: 37-45.
    Ginsburg HJ, Fling S, Hope ML, Musgrove D, Andrews C. 1979. Maternal holding preferences: a consequence of newborn head-turning response. Child Development, 50: 280-281.
    Greenwood PJ. 1980. Mating systems, philopatry, and dispersal in birds and mammals. Animal Behaviour, 28: 1140-1162.
    Guo ST, Li BG, Watanabe K. 2007. Diet and activity budget of Rhinopithecus roxellana in the Qinling Mountains, China. Primates, 48: 268-276.
    
    Haddow AJ. 1952. Field and laboratory studies on an African monkey, Cercopithecus ascanius schmidti Matschie. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 122: 297-394.
    Haraiwa M. 1981. Maternal and alloparental care in a troop of free-ranging Japanese monkeys. Primates, 22: 309-329.
    Harcourt AH. 1978. Strategies of emigration and transfer by primates, with particular reference to gorillas. Zeitschrift Für Tierpsycholog, 48: 401-420.
    Harrison KE, Byrne RW. 2000. Hand preferences in unimanual and bimanual feeding by wild vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 14: 13-21.
    Hart S, Gabbard C. 1997. Examining the stabilizing characteristics of footedness. Laterality, 2: 17-26.
    Hauser MD, Perry S, Manson J, Ball H, William M, Pearson E, Berard J. 1991. It's all in the hands of the beholder: new data on handedness in a free-ranging population of rhesus macaques. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14: 342-344.
    Heestand JE. 1986. Behavioral lateralization in four species of apes? Ph.D. Dissertation. Seattle. University of Washington.
    Henzi SP, Lawes M. 1987. Breeding season influxes and the behavior of adult male samango monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis albogularis). Folia Primatologica, 48: 125-136.
    Hinde RA, Rowell TE, Spencer-Booth Y. 1964. Behavior of socially living rhesus monkeys in their first 6 months. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 143: 609-649.
    Hook-Costigan MA. 2004. The evolution of lateralized motor functions. In: Rogers LJ, Kaplan G. eds. Comparative Vertebrate Cognition: Are Primates Superior to non-primates? Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects (Vol. 3). New York. Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Publishers. pp. 325-370.
    Hook MA, Rogers LJ. 2002. Leading-limb preferences in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): walking, leaping and landing. Laterality, 7: 145-162.
    Hopkins WD. 1999. On the other hand: statistical issues in the assessment and interpretation of hand preference data in nonhuman primates. International Journal of Primatology, 20: 851-866.
    Hopkins WD. 2004. Laterality in materality cradling and infant positional biases: implications for the development and evolution of hand preferences in nonhuman primates. International Journal of Primatology, 25: 1243-1265.
    Hopkins WD. 2007. The evolution of hemispheric specialization in primates. San Diego. Academic Press.
    Hopkins WD. 2008. Brief communication: locomotor limb preferences in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Implications for morphological asymmetries in limb bones. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 137: 113-118.
    Hopkins WD, Bard KA, Bales SL. 1993. Chimpanzee hand preference in throwing and infant cradling: implications for the origin of human handedness. Current Anthropology, 34: 786-790.
    Hopkins WD, de Waal FBM. 1995. Behavioral laterality in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus): replication and extension. International Journal of Primatology, 16: 261-276.
    Hopkins WD, Fernandez-Carriba S. 2002. Laterality in communicative behaviors in nonhuman primates: a critical analysis. In: Rogers L, Andrew R. eds. Comparative Vertebrate Lateralization. Oxford. Oxford University Press. pp. 445-479.
    Hopkins WD, Lathouwers MD. 2006. left nipple preference in infant Pan panicus and P. troglodytes. International Journal of Primatology, 27: 1653-1662.
    Hopkins WD, Russell JL, Remkus M, Freeman H, Schapiro SJ. 2007. Handedness and grooming in Pan troglodytes: comparative analysis between findings in captive and wild individual. International Journal of Primatology, 28: 1315-1326.
    Hrdy S. 1977. The Langurs of Abu. Cambridge. Harvard University Press.
    Isbell LA, van Vuren D. 1996. Differential costs of locational and social dispersal and their consequences for female group-living primates. Behaviour, 133: 1-36.
    Isbell LA, Young TP. 2002. Ecological models of female social relationships in primates: similarities, disparities, and some directions for future clarity. Behaviour, 139: 177-202.
    Jaffe BD, Evans TA, Howell S, Westergaard GC, Snoy PJ, Higley JD. 2006. left versus right nipple preference in free-ranging infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatto). Developmental Psychobiology, 48: 266-272.
    Janson CH, van Schaik CP. 1988. Recognizing the many faces of primate food competition: methods. Behaviour, 105: 165-186.
    Janson CH, Goldsmith ML. 1995. Predicting group size in primates: foraging costs and predation costs. Behavioral Ecology, 6: 326-336.
    Jones CB. 1980. The functions of status in the mantled howler monkey, Alouatta palliata GRAY: intra-specific competition for group membership in a folivorous Neotropical primate. Primates, 21: 389-405.
    Kaplan-Solms KL, Saling MM. 1988. Lateral asymmetry and tactile sensitivity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 67: 55-62.
    Kappeler PM. 1997. Intrasexual selection in Mirza coquereli: evidence for scramble competition polygyny in a solitary primate. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 45: 115-127.
    Koenig A. 2000. Competitive regimes in forest-dwelling hanuman langur females (Semnopithecus entellus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 48: 93-109.
    Koenig A. 2002. Competition for resources and its behavioral consequences among female primates. International Journal of Primatology, 23: 759-783.
    Koenig A, Larney E, Lu A, Borries C. 2004. Agonistic behavior and dominance relationships in female phayre's leaf monkeys-preliminary results. American Journal of Primatology, 64:351-357.
    Korstjens AH, Schippers EP. 2003. Dispersal patterns among olive colobus in Tai national park. International Journal of Primatology, 24: 515-539.
    Korstjens AH, Nijssen EC, Noe R. 2005. Intergroup relationships in western black-and-white colobus, Colobus polykomos polykomos. International Journal of Primatology, 26: 1267-1289.
    
    Krause J, Ruxton GD. 2002. Living in Groups. Oxford. Oxford University Press.
    Kudo H, Dunbar RIM. 2001. Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Animal Behaviour, 62:711-722.
    Kumar A. 1985. Female reproductive success and group size in the lion-tailed macaque (Macaca silenus). Primate Eye, 26: 9.
    Larson CF, Dodson DL, Ward JP. 1989. Hand preferences and whole-body turning biases of lesser bushbabies (Galago senegalensis). Brain Behavior and Evolution, 33: 261-267.
    Lawson Handley LJ, Perrin N. 2007. Advances in our understanding of mammalian sex-biased dispersal. Molecular Ecology, 16: 1559-1578.
    Lhota S. 1999. Situation dependent proceptivity in females of hanuman langur (Presbytis entellus)-the effect of a male takeover. Advance in Ethology, 34: 170.
    Li BG, Chen C, Ji WH, Ren BP. 2000. Seasonal home range changes of the Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains of China. Folia Primatologica, 71: 375-386.
    Li BG, Zhao DP. 2007. Copulation behavior within one-male groups of wild Rhinopithecus roxellana in the Qinling Mountains of China. Primates, 48: 190-196.
    Liang B, Zhang SY. 1998. Hand preference in Sichuan golden monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Acta Theriologica Sinica, 18:107- 111.
    Lindburg DG. 1971. The rhesus monkey in North India: an ecological and behavioral study. In: Roseblum LA. ed. Primate Behavior: Developments in Field and Laboratory Research (Vol. 2). New York. Academic Press, pp. 1-106.
    
    Lomnicki A. 1988. Population Ecology of Individuals. Princeton, Princeton University Press.
    MacNeilage PF. 1991. The "postural origin" theory of primate neurobiological asymmetries. In: Krasnegor NA, Rumbaugh DM, Schieflbusch RL. eds. Biological and Behavioral Determinants of Language Development. Hillsdale. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 165-188.
    MacNeilage PF. 2007. Present status of the postural origins theory. In: Hopkins WD. ed. The Evolution of Hemispheric Specialization in Primates. San Diego. Academic Press. pp 59-91.
    MacNeilage PF, Studdert-Kennedy MG, Lindblom B. 1987. Primate handedness reconsidered. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10: 247-303.
    Maestripieri D. 1994. Social structure, infant handling, and mothering styles in group-living Old World monkeys. International Journal of Primatology, 15: 531-553.
    Majolo B, de Bortoli A, Schino G. 2008. Costs and benefits of group living in primates: group size effects on behaviour and demography. Animal Behaviour, 76: 1235-1247.
    Maki S. 1990. Postural factors in human functional asymmetries. Ph.D. Dissertation. Austin. University of Texas.
    Mann J, Coonor RC, Barre LM, Heithaus MR. 2000. Female reproductive success in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.): life history habitat, provisioning, and group-size effects. Behavioral Ecology, 11: 210-219.
    Marsh CW. 1979. Female transfer and mate choice among Tana River red colobus. Nature, 281: 568-569.
    Martin P, Bateson P. 2007. Measuring Behaviour (the 3rd edition). Cambridge. Cambridge University Press.
    Matsumura S, Okamoto K. 1998. Frequent harassment of mounting after a takeover of a group of moor macaques (Macaca mounts). Primates, 39: 225-230.
    McGrew WC, Marchant LF. 1997. On the other hand: current issues in and meta-analysis of behavioral laterality of hand function in primates. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40:201-232.
    McGrew WC, Marchant LF. 2001. Ethological study of manual laterality in the chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Behaviour, 138: 329-358.
    Michel GF. 1981. right-handedness: a consequence of infant supine head-orientation preference? Science, 212: 685-687.
    Mittra ES, Fuentes A, McGrew WC. 1997. Lack of hand preference in wild hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 103: 455-461.
    Monard AM, Duncan P. 1996. Consequences of natal dispersal in female horses. Animal Behaviour, 52: 565-579.
    Moller LM, Beheregaray LB. 2004. Genetic evidence for sex-biased dispersal in resident bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). Molecular Ecology, 13: 1607-1612.
    Moore J. 1984. Female transfer in primates. International Journal of Primatology, 5: 537-589.
    Morcillo A, Fernadez-carriba S, Loeches A. 2006. Asymmetries in postural control and locomotion in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology, 68: 802-811.
    Mori U, Dunbar RIM. 1985. Changes in the reproductive condition of female gelada baboons following the takeover of one-male units. Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsycholog, 67: 215-224.
    Murai T, Mohamed M, Bernard H, Mahedi PA, Saburi R, Higashi S. 2007. Female transfer between one-male groups of proboscis monkey. Primates, 48: 117-121.
    Nakamichi M. 1983. Development of infant twin Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) in a free-ranging group. Primates, 24: 576-583.
    Neves A, Dolhinow P. 1995. Hand preference in a colobine monkey (Presbytis entellus). American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 38 (Suppl 20): 160.
    Newton PN. 1986. Infanticide in an undisturbed forest population of hunuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Animal Behaviour, 34: 785-789.
    Nias RC, Ford HA. 1992. The influence of group size and habitat on reproductive success in the superb fairy-wren Malurus cyaneus. Emu, 92: 238-243.
    Nicholson AJ. 1954. An outline of the dynamics of animal populations. Australian Journal of Zoology, 2: 9-65.
    Nishida T. 1993. left nipple suckling preference in wild chimpanzees. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14: 45-51.
    Ohsawa H. 2003. Long-term study of the social dynamics of patas monkeys (Erythrocebiis patas): group male supplanting and changes to the multi-male situation. Primates, 44: 99-107.
    Ota K, Makino Y, Kimura M, Suzuki J. 1991. Lactation in the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata): yield and composition of milk and nipple preference of young. Primates, 32: . 35-48.
    Panger MA. 1998. Hand preference in free-ranging white-throated capuchins (Cebus capucinus) in Costa Tica. International Journal of Primatology, 19: 133-163.
    Papademetriou E, Sheu CF, Michel GF. 2005. A meta-analysis of primate hand preferences, particularly for reaching. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119: 33-48.
    
    Paul A. 2002. Sexual selection and mate choice. International Journal of Primatology, 23: 877-904.
    Paul A, Preuschoft S, van Schaik CP. 2000. The other side of the coin: infanticide and the evolution of affiliative male-infant interactions in Old World primates. In: van Schaik CP, Janson CH. eds. Infanticide by Males and Its Implications. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 269-292.
    Pawlowski BP, I owen CB, Dunbar RIM. 1998. Neocortex size, social skills and mating success in primates. Behaviour, 135 357-368.
    Perrin N, Mazalov V. 1999. Dispersal and inbreeding avoidance. American Naturalist, 154: 282-292.
    Peters M. 1988. Footedness: asymmetries in foot preference and skill and neuropsychological assessment of foot movement. Psychological Bulletin, 103: 179-192.
    Petit E, Balloux F, Goudet J. 2001. Sex-biased dispersal in a migratory bat: a characterization using sex-specific demographic parameters. Evolution, 55: 635-640.
    Pope T. 2000. The evolutionary of male philopatry in neotropical primates. In: Kappeler PM. ed. Primate Males: Causes and Consequences of Variation in Group Composition. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 219-235.
    
    Porac C, Coren S. 1981. Lateral Preferences and Human Behavior. New York. Springer.
    Pusey AE. 1992. The primate perspective on dispersal. In: Stenseth NC, Lidicker Jr WZ. eds. Animal Dispersal: Small Mammals as a Model. London. Chapman and Hall. pp. 243-259.
    Pusey AE, Packer C. 1987. Dispersal and philopatry. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT. eds. Primate Societies. Chicago. University of Chicago Press. pp. 250-266.
    Pusey A, Wolf M. 1996. Inbreeding avoidance in animals. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 11:201-206.
    Rajpurohit LS, Chhangani AK, Rajpurohit RS, Mohnot SM. 2003. Observation of a sudden resident-male replacement in a unimale bisexual troop of hanuman langurs, Semnopithecus entellus, around Jodhpur (India). Folia Primatologica, 74: 85-87.
    Raymond M, Pontier D. 2004. Is there geographical variation in human handedness? Laterality, 9: 35-52.
    Redmond JC, Lamperez A. 2004. Leading limb preference during brachiation in the gibbon family member, Hylobates syndactylus (siamangs): a study of the effect of singing on lateralization. Laterality, 9: 381-396.
    Reena M, Ram MB. 1992. Rate of takeovers in groups of hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) at Jaidpur. Folia Primatologica, 58: 61-71.
    Reichard U. 1995. Extra-pair copulations in a monogamous gibbon (Hylobates lar). Ethology, 100:99-112.
    Ren RM, Yan KH, Su YJ, Qi HJ, Liang B, Bao WY. 1995. The reproductive behavior of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys in captivity (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Primates, 36: 135-143.
    
    Reynolds J. 1996. Animal breeding systems. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 11: 68-72.
    Robbins AM, Stoinski TS, Fawcett KA, Robbins MM. 2009. Socioecological influence on the dispersal of female mountain gorillas-evidence of a second folivore paradox. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63: 477-489.
    Robbins MM, Robbins AM, Gerald-Steklis N, Steklis HD. 2007. Socioecological influences on the reproductive success of female mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61: 919-931.
    Robinson JG. 1988. Group size in wedge-capped capuchin monkeys Cebus olivaceus and the reproductive success of males and females. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61: 187-197.
    Rogers LJ, Kaplan G. 1996. Hand preference and other lateral biases in rehabilitated orangutans, Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus. Animal Behaviour, 51: 13-25.
    Ross C. 1993. Takeover and infanticide in South Indian hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). American Journal of Primatology, 30: 75-82.
    Rudran R. 1973. Adult male replacement in one-male troops of purple-faced langurs (Presbytis senex senex) and its effect on population structure. Folia Primatologica, 19: 166-192.
    Saj TL, Sicotte P. 2005. Male takeover in Colobus vellerosus at Boabeng-Fiema monkey sanctuary, Central Ghana. Primates, 46: 211-214.
    Saj TL, Marteinson S, Chapman CA, Sicotte P. 2007. Controversy over the application of current socioecological models to folivorous primates: Colobus vellerosus fits the predictions. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 133: 994-1003.
    Saling MM, Cooke W. 1984. Cradling and transport of infants by South African mothers: a cross-cultural study. Current Anthropology, 25: 333-335.
    Salk L. 1973. The role of the heartbeat in the relations between mother and infant. Scientific American, 228: 24-29.
    Schubert G. 1982. Infanticide by usurper hanuman langur males: a sociobiological myth. Social Science Information, 21: 199-214.
    
    Searleman A. 1980. Subject variables and cerebral organization. Cortex, 16: 239-254.
    Seltzer C, Forsythe C, Ward JP. 1990. Multiple measures of motor lateralization in human primates (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104: 159-166.
    Sicotte P, Terchroeb JA, Saj TL. 2007. Aspects of male competition in Colobus vellerosus: preliminary data on male and female loud calling, and infant deaths after a takeover. International Journal of Primatology, 28: 627-636.
    Silk JB. 2007. The adaptive value of sociality in mammalian groups. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society of London Series B, 362: 539-559.
    
    Smuts BB. 1987. Sexual competition and mate choice. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT. eds. Primate Societies. Chicago. University of Chicago Press. pp. 385-399.
    Snaith TV, Chapman CA. 2007. Primate group size and interpreting socioecological models: do folivores really play by different rules? Evolutionary Anthropology, 16: 94-106.
    Snaith TV, Chapman CA. 2008. Red colobus monkeys display alternative behavioral responses to the costs of scramble competition. Behavioral Ecology, 19: 1289-1296.
    Sprague DS, Suzuki S, Tsukuhara T. 1996. Variation in social mechanisms by which males attained the alpha rank among Japanese macaques. In: Fa JE, Lindburg DG. eds. Evolution and Ecology of Macaque Societies. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 444-458.
    Stafford DK, Milliken GW, Ward JP. 1990. Lateral bias in feeding and brachiation in Hylobates. Primates, 31: 407-414.
    Steenbeek R. 2000. Infanticide by males and female choice in wild Thomas's langurs. In: van Schaik CP, Janson CH. eds. Infanticide by Males and Its Implications. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 153-177.
    Steenbeek R, Sterck EHM, de Vries H, van Hooff JARAM. 2000. Costs and benefits of the one-male, age-graded, and all-male phases in wild Thomas's langur groups. In: Kappeler PM. ed. Primate Males: Causes and Consequences of Variation in Group Composition. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 130-145.
    Steenbeek R, van Schaik CP. 2000. Competition and group size in Thomas's langurs (Presbytis thomasi): the folivore paradox revisited. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 49: 100-110.
    Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP. 1997. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41: 291-309.
    Sterck EHM, Korstjens AM. 2000. Female dispersal and infanticide avoidance in primates. In: van Schaik CP, Janson CH. eds. Infanticide by Males and Its Implications. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 293-321.
    Sterck EHM, Willems EP, van Hooff JARAM, Wich SA. 2005. Female dispersal, inbreeding avoidance and mate choice in Thomas langurs (Presbytis thomasi). Behaviour, 142: 845-868.
    Stokes EJ, Parnell RJ, Olejniczak C. 2003. Female dispersal and reproductive success in wild western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 54: 329-339.
    Strier KB. 2000. From binding brotherhoods to short-term sovereignty: the dilemma of male Cebidae. In: Kappeler PM. ed. Primate Males: Causes and Consequences of Variation in Group Composition. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 72-83.
    Struhsaker TT. 1977. Infanticide and social organization in the redtail monkey (Cercopithecus ascanius schmidti) in the Kibale Forest. Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsycholog, 45: 75-84.
    Sugiyama Y. 1966. An artificial social change in a hanuman langur group (Presbytis entellus). Primates 7: 41-72.
    Swedell L. 2000. Two takeovers in wild hamadryas baboons. Folia Primatologica, 71: 169-172.
    Takahata Y, Suzuki S, Okayasu N, Sugiura H, Izawa K, Agetsuma N, Hill D, Saito C, Sato S, Tanaka T, Sprague DS. 1998. Does troop size of wild Japanese macaques influence birth rate and infant morality in the absence of predator? Primates, 39: 245-251.
    Takahata Y, Koyma N, Ichino S, Miyamoto N, Nakamichi M. 2006. Influence of group size on reproductive success of female ring-tailed lemurs: distinguishing between IGFC and PFC hypotheses. Primates, 47: 383-387.
    Tan CL, Guo ST, Li BG. 2007. Population structure and ranging patterns of Rhinopithecus roxellana in Zhouzhi National Nature Reserve, Shaanxi, China. International Journal of Primatology, 28: 577-591.
    Tanaka I. 1989. Change of nipple preference between successive offspring in Japanese macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 18: 321-325.
    Tanaka I. 1997. Parity-related differences in suckling behavior and nipple preference among free-ranging Japanese macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 42: 331-339.
    Teichroeb JA, Sicotte P. 2009. Test of the ecological-constraints model on ursine colobus monkeys (Colobus vellerosus) in Ghana. American Journal of Primatology, 71: 49-59.
    Tokuda K. 1969. On the handedness of Japanese monkeys. Primates, 10: 41-46.
    Tomaszycki M, Cline C, Griffin B, Maestripieri D, Hopkins WD. 1998. Maternal cradling and infant nipple preference in rhesus macaques. Developmental Psychobiology, 32: 305-312.
    Tommasi L, Vallortigara G. 1999. Footednes in binocular and monocular chicks. Laterality, 4: 89-95.
    Trivers RL. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campell B. ed. Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man. Chicago. Aldine. pp 136-179.
    Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ. 2005. Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28: 575-633.
    van Belle S, Estrada A. 2008. Group size and composition influence male and female reproductive success in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). American Journal of Primatology, 70: 613-619.
    van Noordwijk MA, van Schaik CP. 2001. Career moves: transfer and rank challenge decisions by male long-tailed macaques. Behaviour, 138: 359-395.
    
    van Schaik CP. 1983. Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour, 87: 120-144.
    van Schaik CP. 1989. The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In: Standen V, Foley RA. eds. Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioural Ecology of Humans and Other Mammals. Oxford, Blackwell. pp. 195-218.
    van Schaik CP. 2000a. Infanticide by male primates; the sexual selection hypothesis revisited. In: van Schaik CP, Janson CH. eds. Infanticide by Males and Its Implications. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 27-60.
    van Schaik CP. 2000b. Social strategies against infanticide by males in primates and other mammals. In: Kappeler PM. ed. Primate Males: Causes and Consequences of Variation in Group Composition. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 35-52.
    Vauclair J, Donnot J. 2005. Infant holding biases and their relations to hemispheric specializations for perceiving facial emotions. Neuropsychologia, 43: 564-571.
    Wang HP, Tan CL, Gao YF, Li BG. 2004. A takeover of resident male in the Sichuan snub-nosed monkey Rhinopithecus roxellana in Qinling Mountains. Acta Zoologica Sinica, 50: 859-862.
    Ward JP, Hopkins WD. 1993. Primate Laterality: Current Behavioral Evidence of Primate Asymmetries. New York. Springer.
    Wan-en JM. 1980. Handedness and laterality in humans and other animals. Physiological Psychology, 8: 351-359.
    Warren JM, Abplanalp JM, Warren HB. 1967. The development of handedness in cats and rhesus monkeys. In: Stephenson HW, Hess EH, Reingold HL. eds. Early Behaviour: Comparative and Developmental Approaches. New York. Wiley. pp. 103-119.
    Watanabe K, Kawai M. 1993. Lateralized hand use in the precultural behavior of Koshima monkeys (Macaca fuscata). In: Ward JP, Hopkins WD. eds. Primate Laterality-Current Behavioral Evidence of Primate Asymmetries. New York. Springer. ppl83-192.
    Watanabe K, Wada K, Li JH. 2007. Hand preference in Macaca thibetana. Acta Zoologica Sinica, 53: 570-574
    Watts DP. 1990. Ecology of gorillas and its relation to female transfer in mountain gorillas. International Journal of Primatology, 11: 21-45.
    Wells DL. 2002. Hand preference for feeding in captive Colobus guereza. Folia Primatologica, 73: 57-59.
    Westergaard GC, Kuhn HE, Suomi SJ .1998. Bipedal posture and hand preference in humans and other primates. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112: 56-63.
    Wheatley BP. 1982. Adult male replacement in Macaca fasicularis of East Kalimantan, Indonesia. International Journal of Primatology, 3: 203-219.
    Winkler P. Prestel H. 1989. Twins in free-ranging hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Primates, 30: 255-259.
    Wolfe KE, Fleagle JG. 1977. Adult male replacement in a group of silvered leaf monkeys (Presbytis cristata) at Kuala Selangor, Malaysia. Primates, 18: 949-955.
    Wrangham RW. 1980. An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour, 75: 262-300.
    Xi WZ, Li BG, Zhao DP, Ji WH, Zhang P. 2008. Benefits to female helpers in wild Rhinopithecus roxellana. International Journal of Primatology, 29: 593-600.
    Yan CE, Jiang ZG. 2006. Does estradiol modulate sexual solicitations in female Rhinopithecus roxellana! International Journal of Primatology, 27: 1171-1186.
    Zhang P, Watanabe K, Li BG, Tan CL. 2006. Social organization of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains, central China. Primates, 47: 374-382.
    Zhang SY, Liang B, Wang LX. 2000. Seasonality of matings and births in captive Sichuan golden monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). American Journal of Primatology, 51: 265-269.
    Zhang SY, Liang B, Wang LX. 1999. Infanticide within captive groups of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Folia Primatologica, 70: 274-276.
    Zhao DP, Li BG, Li YH, Wada K. 2005. Extra-unit sexual behavior among wild Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains of China. Folia Primatologica, 76: 172-176.
    Zhao DP, Li BG, Groves CP, Watanabe K. 2008a. Impact of male takeover on intra-unit sexual interactions and subsequent interbirth interval in wild Rhinopithecus roxellana. Folia Primatologica, 79: 93-102.
    Zhao DP, Ji WH, Li BG, Watanabe K. 2008b. Mate competition and reproductive con-elates of female dispersal in a polygynous primate species (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Behavioural Processes, 79: 165-170.
    Zhao DP, Ji WH, Watanabe K, Li BG. 2008c. Hand preference of unimanual and bimanual reaching actions in Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). American Journal of Primatology, 70: 500-504.
    Zhao DP, Li BG, Watanabe K. 2008d. First evidence on foot preference during locomotion in Old World monkeys: a study of quadrupedal and bipedal actions in Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Primates, 49: 260-264.
    Zhao DP, Gao X, Li BG, Watanabe K. 2008e. First wild evidence of neonate nipple preference and maternal cradling laterality in Old World monkeys: a preliminary study from Rhinopithecus roxellana. Behavioural Processes, 77: 364-368.
    Zinner D, Deschner T. 2000. Sexual swelling in female hamadryas baboons after male take-overs: "deceptive" swellings as a possible female counter-strategy against infanticide. American Journal of Primatology, 52: 157-168.
    Zumpe D, Michael RP. 1986. Dominance index: a simple measure of relative dominance status in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 10: 291-301.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700