个体人格特质对跨期决策的影响及其神经基础
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
跨期决策是指个体对发生在不同时间点的成本与收益进行权衡,进而做出的各种判断和选择。现实生活中像这样在现在和未来之间或在不同时间点之间做出权衡抉择的例子举不胜举。小到个体的日常生活中饮食选择,大到群体组织乃至国家、跨国组织的公共政策的制定,人类的许多决策都是基于时间维度对成本和利益的权衡而做出的。研究跨期决策对于探讨动物和人类的心智起源和发展过程,搞清现实生活中如烟草和酒精成瘾、吸毒行为、病理性赌博成瘾、孤独症、注意缺陷多动症、暴食暴饮症等一系列行为障碍和心理精神疾病的起因具有重要的理论价值;对现实生活中更加理性决策个人的教育、饮食习惯、健康、婚姻生育、退休储蓄、投资以及国家的经济宏观调控和可持续发展问题,甚至全球气候和环境的决策具有非凡的现实意义。
     基于以前的研究,本研究从人格特质差异角度出发,利用问卷调查法、行为研究法和脑功能成像法(fMRI)相结合的研究手段,通过6个实验系统地探讨了自我控制、未来时间洞察力、时间感知在跨期决策中的影响作用及其神经基础。
     在研究一中,采用问卷调查法初步探察了自我控制、冲动性、未来时间洞察力与跨期选择中延迟折扣的关系。结果表明被试在延迟折扣任务中无差异点大小(indifference)与自我控制、未来时间洞察力的自我评分呈正相关,与冲动性的自我评分呈负相关。
     在研究二的实验1和实验2中,利用跨期选择任务和脑功能成像技术考察了自我控制能力不同群体(高自我控制者、低自我控制者)在跨期选择中反应倾向差异及其相应的脑神经反应活动的差异。行为结果表明,进行跨期选择时,无论任务难易,低控者选择即时兑现但较小奖赏的概率比高控者明显高;脑功能成像结果表明,在右侧额下回/额中回(BA45/46)、右侧颞上回/颞中回/额下回(BA38/47)、右侧海马旁回/梭状回(BA37/20/36/35)、左侧颞中回/额下回/梭状回(BA21/20)、左侧梭状回/舌回(BA18)、海马旁回、额内侧回(BA32)、右侧舌回(BA17)、右侧颞下回/枕中回(BA19/37)、左侧颞中回/颞上回(BA21/22)、右侧颞上回(BA42)、右侧颞中回/颞上回(BA39/40)、左侧颞中回/颞上回(BA39/19)、左侧中央后回(BA5/7)、右侧中央后回(BA2/3)等区域高控者明显明显比低控者激活程度强烈。
     在研究三的实验3和实验4中,利用跨期选择任务和脑功能成像技术考察了未来时间洞察力不同群体(高时间洞察者、低时间洞察者)在跨期选择中反应倾向差异及其相应的脑神经反应活动的差异。行为结果表明,跨期选择中低时间洞察力者进行选择即时兑现但较小奖赏的概率比高时间洞察力者明显高;脑功能成像结果表明,在左侧海马旁回(BA28/36)、左侧梭状回/枕中回/枕下回(BA18/17)、左侧梭状回(BA37)、左侧额内侧回(BA6)、左侧颞中回(BA39/19)、左侧扣带回(BA7/31)、左侧颞上回/海马旁回/海马回(BA28/34)等区域高时间洞察力者明显比低时间洞察力者激活程度强烈。
     在研究四的实验5和实验6中,利用时间估计任务、跨期选择任务和脑功能成像技术考察了不同时间感知群体(时间高估者、时间低估者)在跨期选择中反应倾向差异及其相应的脑神经反应活动的差异。行为结果表明,和时间高估者被试相比,无论任务难易,时间低估者更倾向于延迟兑现但较大的奖赏选项;脑功能成像结果表明,在左侧海马旁回(BA34)、右侧颞上回/颞中回(BA38)、左侧梭状回(BA37)、右侧枕中回(BA18)、左侧额中回/额上回(BA47)、左侧枕中回(BA19)、左侧顶上小叶/顶下小叶/楔前叶/海马旁回(BA19/7)、左侧楔叶(BA17)、左侧额中回/额下回(BA6)、丘脑、右侧额下回(BA9)、左侧额上回/额中回(BA9/10)、右侧额上回/额中回/扣带回(BA6/32)、右侧楔前叶(BA7)、左侧额上回(BA6)等区域,时间低估者明显比时间高估者被试的激活程度强烈。
     总之,本研究发现,自我控制、未来时间洞察力、时间感知在跨期决策甲起着重要的调控影响作用,这些能力的差异一定程度上很可能是诱发跨期选择偏向个体差异的重要原因。未来的研究需要进一步搞清这些影响因素在跨期选择中如何精细调控个体选择倾向的神经机制。
Intertemporal choices are decisions that involve tradeoffs among costs and benefits occurring at different times. In our real world, there are numerous examples about intertemporal choices. These choices, for example, range from the prosaic-which and how much food to eat at a meal-to life-changing vital decisions about public policy made by states and multinational organizations. In the one hand, the researches of intertemporal choice have important theoretical value. Firstly, it can exploit the mental origin and development process of animals and human. Secondly, it can help to find out the causes of a series of behavior disorder and psychological disease, such as tabacoo addiction, alcohol addiction, drug abuse, pathological gambling addiction, autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and binge eating disorder. In the other hand, the researches of intertemporal choice also have important realistic applications. For example, it can make people more rational decision making in personal education, dietary habits, health, marriage, fertility and family retirement savings, investment and the country's economic macro regulation and the sustainable development of the world, or even the world's climate and environment decision problem.
     Based on previous studies, the thesis studied intertemporal choice from the perspective of personality trait. Combining the questionnaire survey, behavioral research, and fMRI researches, the thesis systematically invested the impact of self-control, future temporal perspective, and time perception on intertemporal choice and their neural basis using intertemporal choice tasks in six experiments.
     Study1preliminary probed into the relationship between delay discounting and self-control, impulsive and future temporal perspective by a questionnaire survey. The results showed that there are significantly positive relationships between individuals' indifferences and the self ratings of self-control and future temporal perspective, whereas there is a significantly negative relationship between individuals'indifferences and the self ratings of impulsive.
     In study2, experiment1and experiment2examined the choice difference of different groups which have different ability of self-control (high self-control group vs. low self-control group) and their neural response difference in intertemporal choice by fMRI. Behavioral results showed that, whether the choice task is difficult or easy, low self-control group chose more smaller-immediate rewards than high self-control group. fMRI results showed that high self-control group have higher activation than low self-control group in Right Inferior/middle Frontal Gyrus(BA45/46), Right Superior/middle Temporal Gyrus/Inferior Frontal Gyrus(BA38/47), Right Parahippocampal Gyrus/Fusiform Gyrus(BA37/20/36/35), Left Middle/Inferior Temporal Gyrus/Fusiform Gyrus(BA21/20), Left Fusiform/Lingual Gyrus(BA18), Parahippocampa Gyrus, Medial Frontal Gyrus(BA32), Right Lingual Gyrus(BA17), Right Inferior Temporal Gyrus/Middle Occipital Gyrus(BA21/22), Left Middle/Superior Temporal Gyrus(BA21/22), Right Superior Temporal Gyrus(BA42), Right Middle/Superior Temporal Gyrus(BA39/40), Left Postcentral Gyrus(BA5/7), Right Postcentral Gyrus (BA2/3), Left Middle Temporal/Superior Gyrus(BA39/19).
     In study3, experiment3and experiment4examined the choice difference of different groups which have different ability of future temporal perspective (high future temporal perspective group vs. low future temporal perspective group) and their neural response difference in intertemporal choice by fMRI. Behavioral results showed that whether the task is difficult or easy, low future temporal perspective group chose more smaller-immediate rewards than high future temporal perspective group. fMRI results showed that high future temporal perspective group have higher activation than low future temporal perspective group in Middle Frontal Gyrus(BA6), Fusiform Gyrus Middle/Inferior Occipital Gyrus(BA18/17), Fusiform Gyrus(BA37), Superior Temporal GyrusPara/hippocampa Gyrus(BA28/34), Middle Temporal Gyrus(BA39/19), Cingulate Gyrus(BA7/31), Parahippocampa Gyrus(BA28/36).
     In study4, experiment5and experiment6examined the choice difference of different groups which have different ability of time perception (high time perception group vs. low time perception group) and their neural response difference in intertemporal choice by fMRI. Behavioral results showed that whether the task is difficult or easy, low time perception group chose more larger-delayed rewards than high time perception group. fMRI results showed that low time perception group have higher activation than high time perception group in Left ParaHippocampal Gyrus(BA34), Right Superior/Middle Temporal Gyrus(BA38), Left Fusiform Gyru(BA37), Right Middle Occipital Gyrus(BA18), Left Middle/Inferior Frontal Gyrus(BA47), Left Middle Occipital Gyrus(BA19), Left Superior/Inferior Parietal Lobule/Precuneus/Parahippocampa Gyrus(BA19/7), Left Cuneus(BA17), Left Middle/Inferior Frontal Gyrus(BA6), Thalamus, Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus (BA9), Left Superior/Middle Frontal Gyrus(BA9/10), Left Superior Frontal Gyrus(BA6), Right Precuneus(BA7), Right Superior/Middle Frontal Gyrus/Cingulate Gyrus(BA6/32).
     To sum up, the present researches found that self-control, future temporal perspective, and time perception may play important roles in intertemporal choices. The differences of these ability factors may be the important reasons of the differences of individuals'choice and the differences of the neural activation. Future researches will further exams how these factors modulate individuals'choice response at neurological level in intertemporal choices.
引文
陈海贤,何贵兵.(2011).识解水平对跨期选择和风险选择的影响.心理学报,442-452.
    何嘉梅,黄希庭.(2009).时间贴现的性质与脑机制.心理科学进展,17,98-105.
    黄希庭.(1993).时距信息加工的认知研究.西南师范大学学报(自然科学版),12,207-215.
    黄希庭.(2004).论时间洞察力.心理科学.27,5-7.
    马文娟,索涛,李亚丹,罗笠铢,冯廷勇,李红.(2012).得失框架效应的分离:来自收益与损失型跨期选择的研究.心理学报,(in press).
    马谐,陶云,胡文钦.(2009).时距知觉中的情绪效应.心理科学进展,17,29-36.
    谭树华.(2009).可—不在可:自制力消耗效应研究.华中师范大学硕士学位论文,武汉.
    谭树华,郭永玉.(2008).大学生自我控制量表的修订.中国临床心理学杂志,16(5),468-470.
    宋其争.(2004).大学生未来时间洞察力的理论和实证研究.西南师范大学博士学位论文,重庆.
    王鹏,刘永芳.(2009).情绪对跨时选择的影响.心理科学,32,1318-1320.
    张志杰,黄希庭.(2007).时距估计年龄差异的认知机制.心理科学,30,524-528.
    周亮,肖水源,何晓燕,厉洁,刘慧铭.(2006).BIS-11中文版的信度与效度检验.中国临床心·理学杂志,14,342-344.
    Acheson, A., Farrar, A. M., Patak, M., Hausknecht, K. A., Kieres, A. K., Choi, S., Wit, H. de & Richards, J. B. (2006). Nucleus accumbens lesions decrease sensitivity to rapid changes in the delay to reinforcement. Behavioural Brain Research,173.217-228.
    Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M. A., Laiser, N., & Schacter, D. L. (2009). Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past:Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering. Neuropsychologia,47,2222-2238.
    Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Constructive episodic simulation:temporal distance and detail of past and future events modulate hippocampal engagement. Hippocampus,18, 227-237.
    Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the past and imagining the future:Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia,45,1363-1377.
    Aharon, I., et al. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value:fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron,32,537-551.
    Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward, a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol. Bull.82,463-496.
    Ainslie, G. (1992). Picoeconomics:The Strategic Interaction of Successive Motivational States within the Person. New York:Cambridge University Press.
    Angeletos, G., Laibson, D., Repetto, A., Tobacman, J., & Weinberg, S. (2001). The hyperbolic consumption model:Calibration, simulation, and empirical evaluation. Journal of Economic Perspectives,15,47-68.
    Anokhin, A. P., et al. (2011). Heritability of delay discounting in adolescence:a longitudinal twin study. Behav. Genet,41,175-183.
    Ballard, K., & Knutson, B. (2009). Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. Neuroimage,45,143-150.
    Bar, M. (2009). The proactive brain:memory for predictions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci,364,1235-1243.
    Bar, M. (2010) Wait for the second marshmallow? Future-oriented thinking and delayed reward discounting in the brain. Neuron,66,4-5.
    Bar, M., Aminoff, E., Mason, M., & Fenske, M. (2007). The units of thought. Hippocampus,17, 420-428.
    Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,29,541-556.
    Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal B:Society Biological Science,364,1281-1289.
    Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure:An overview. Psychological Inquiry,7,1-15.
    Baxter, M. G., & Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.,3,563-573.
    Baxter, M. G., Parker, A., Lindner, C. C. C., Izquierdo, A. C., & Murray, E. A. (2000). Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction of amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci.,20,4311-4319.
    Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs:a neurocognitive perspective. Nature neuroscience,8,1458-1463.
    Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000a). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex,10,295-307.
    Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (1998). Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci.,18,428-437.
    Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H. (2000b). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain,123,2189-2202.
    Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex,6, 215-225.
    Becker, G. S. (1997). The endogenous determination of time preference. Quarterly Journal of Economics,112,729-758.
    Benjamin, D. J., Brown, S. A., & Shapiro, J. M. (2008). Who is "behavioral"? Cognitive abilityand anomalous preferences (Working Paper, Harvard University).Retrieved August 4,2008, from http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract id5675264.
    Benoit, R. G., Gilbert,S. J., & Burgess, P. W. (2011). A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. Journal ofNeuroscience,31,6771-6779.
    Benzion, U., Rapoport, A., & Yagil, J. (1989). Discount rates inferred from decisions:an experimental study. Manage Science,35,270-284.
    Berlin, H. A., Rolls, E. T., & Kischka, U. (2004). Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain,127,1108-1126.
    Berns, G. S., et al. (2001). Predictability modulates human brain response to reward. J. Neurosci.. 21,2793-2798.
    Berns, G. S., et al. (2006). Neurobiological substrates of dread. Science,312,754-758.
    Berns, G. S., Laibson, D. & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Intertemporal choice-toward an integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,11,82-48.
    Bjork, J. M., et al. (2009). Delay discounting correlates with proportional lateral frontal cortex volumes. Biol. Psychiatry,65,710-713.
    Bickel, W. K., & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence:Delay discounting processes. Addiction,96,73-86.
    Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking:Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers, Psychopharmacology (Berlin),146, 447-454.
    Bickel, W. K. Pitcock, J. A., Yi, R., & Angtuaco, E. J. C. (2009). Congruence of BOLD Response across Intertemporal Choice Conditions:Fictive and Real Money Gains and Losses. The Journal ofNeuroscience,29,8839-8846.
    Botzung, A., Denkova, E., & Manning, L. (2008). Experiencing past and future events:Functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time travel. Brain and Cognition,66, 202-212.
    Breiter, H. C., et al. (2001) Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 30,619-639.
    Brody, C. D., Hernandez, A., Zainos, A., & Romo, R. (2003). Timing and neural encoding of somatosensory parametric working memory in macaque prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex,13, 1196-1207.
    Bschor, T., et al. (2004). Time experience and time judgment in major depression, mania and healthy subjects. A controlled study of 93 subjects. Acta Psychiatr. Scand.,109,222-229.
    Buckner, R.L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci.,11,49-57.
    Buhusi, C. V., & Meek, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,6,755-765.
    Buhusi, C. V., & Meek, W. H. (2006). Interval timing with gaps and distracters:evaluation of the ambiguity, switch, and time-sharing hypotheses. Animal Behavior Processes,32,329-338.
    Buonomano, D.V., & Karmarkar, U. R. (2002). How do we tell time? Neuroscientist,8,42-51.
    Burle, B., & Casini, L. (2001). Dissociation between activation and attention effects in time estimation:implications for internal clock models. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform,27, 195-205.
    Cappella, B., et al. (1977). Time estimation by hyperactive and normal children. Percept. Mot. Skills,44,787-790.
    Cardinal, R. N. (2006). Neural systems implicated in delayed and probabilistic reinforcement. Neural Networks,19, 1277-1301.
    Cardinal, R. N., Pennicott, D. R., Sugathapala, C. L., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (2001). Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions to the nucleus accumbens core. Science,292, 2499-2501.
    Carter, R. M., Meyer, J. R., Huettel, S. A. (2010). Functional Neuroimaging of Intertemporal Choice Models:A Review. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics,3,27-45.
    Casey, B. J., et al. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later.
    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA,108,14998-15003.
    Casey, B. J., et al. (2008) The adolescent brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.,1124,111-126.
    Chen, H., et al. (2005). Cultural differences in consumer impatience. Journal of marketing research, 42,291-301.
    Cheung, T. H., & Cardinal, R. N. (2005). Hippocampal lesions facilitate instrumental learning with delayed reinforcement but induce impulsive choice in rats. B.M.C. Neurosci,6,36.
    Christakou, A., et al. (2011). Maturation of limbic corticostriatal activation and connectivity associated with developmental changes in temporal discounting. Neuroimage,54,1344-1354.
    Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (2003). A concise introduction to scalar timing theory. In:Meck, W.H. (Ed.), Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing, vol.1. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, pp.3-22.
    Church, R. M., (1984). Properties of the internal clock. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.423.566-582.
    Coller, M., Williams, M. B. (1999). Eliciting individual discount rates. Experimental Economics,2, 107-127.
    Critchfield, T. S. & Madden, G. J. (Eds). (2006). At the crossroads of time and action:A temporal discounting primer for prospective memory researchers, In:Glicksohn J & Myslobodsky M S, Timing the future The case for a time-based prospective memory (pp.117-142). USA:World scientific publishing Co. Pte. Ltd.
    Cromwell, H.C., & Schultz, W. (2003). Effects of expectations for different reward magnitudes on neuronal activity in primate striatum. J. Neurophysiol.,89.2823-2838.
    D'Argembeau. A., Xue, G., Lu, Z. L., Van der Linden, M., & Bechara, A. (2008). Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near and far future. Neurolmage,40.398-407.
    Dasgupta, P., & Maskin, E. (2005). Uncertainty and hyperbolic discounting. The American Economic Review,95,1290-1299.
    Delgado. M. R., et al. (2000). Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. J. Neurophysiol.,84.3072-3077.
    Denk. F., Walton, M. E., Jennings, K. A, Sharp, T., Rushworth, M. F., Bannerman, D. M. (2005). Differential involvement of serotonin and dopamine systems in cost-benefit decisions about delay or effort. Psychopharmacology (Berl).179,587-596.
    Diekamp, B., Kalt, T.,&Gunturkun, O. (2002). Working memory neurons in pigeons. J. Neurosci.,22,1-5.
    Dixon, M. R., Marley, J., & Jacobs, E. A. (2003). Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. J. Appl. Behav. Anal.,36,449-458.
    Dougherty, D. M., et al. (2003). Validation of the immediate and delayed memory tasks in hospitalized adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders. Psychol. Rec.,53,509-533.
    Durstewitz, D. (2003). Self-organizing neural integrator predicts interval times through climbing activity. J. Neurosci.,23,5342-5353.
    Durstewitz, D. (2004). Neural representation of interval time. Neurorepon,15,745-749.
    Eisenberg, D. T. et al. (2007). Examining impulsivity as an endophenotype using a behavioral approach:a DRD2 TaqI A and DRD4 48-bp VNTR association study. Behav. Brain Funct.,3,
    Epstein, L. H., Richards, J. B., Saad, F. G., Paluch, R. A., Roemmich, J. N., & Lerman, C. (2003). Comparison between two measures of delay discounting in smokers. Exp. Clin Psychopharmacol.,11,131-138.
    Estle, S. J., Green, L., Myerson, J., & Holt, D. D. (2007). Discounting of Monetary and Directly Consumable Rewards. Psychological Science, 18, 58-63.
    Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Dissociable elements of human foresight: a role for the ventromedial frontal lobes in framing the future, but not in discounting future rewards. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1214-1221.
    Figner, B., Knoch, D., Johnson, E. J., Krosch, A. R., Lisanby, S. H., & Fehr, E. (2010). Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in inter-temporal choice. Nature neuroscience, 13, 538-539.
    Fillmore, M. T., & Rush, C. R. (2002). unpaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 66, 265-273.
    Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 25-42.
    Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G. F., & O'Donoghue, T., (2002). Time discounting and time preference: a critical review. J. Econ. Lit., 40, 351-401.
    Fudenberg, D., & Levine, D.K. (2006). A dual-self model of impulse control. Am. Econ. Rev., 96, 1449-1476.
    Fuster, J. M. (2000). Executive frontal functions. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 66-70.
    Giedd, J. N. (2004). Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1021, 77-85.
    Giesme, T. (1983). On the concept of future time orientation: Consderations of some functions' and measurements' implications. International Journal of Psychology, 10, 444-460.
    Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: experiencing the future. Science, 317, 1351-1354.
    Glicksohn, J., Leshem, R., & Aharoni, R. (2006). Impulsivity and time estimation: Casting a net to catch a fish. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 261-271.
    Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2002). Drug Addiction and Its Underlying Neurobiological Basis: Neuroimaging Evidence for the Involvement of the Frontal Cortex. American journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1642-1652.
    Green, L., Fry, A., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33-36.
    Green, L., et al. (1996). Temporal discounting in choice between delayed rewards: the role of age and income. Psychol. Aging, 11, 79-84.
    Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 769-792.
    Green, L., Myerson, J., & Ostaszewski, P. (1999a). Amount of reward has opposite effects on the discounting of delayed and probabilistic outcomes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,25,418-427.
    Green, L., Myerson, J., and Ostaszewski, P. (1999b). Discounting of delayed rewards across the life span:age differences in individual discounting functions. Behav. Process.,46,89-96.
    Gregorios-Pippas, L. V., Tobler, P. N., Schultz, W. (2005). Processing of reward delay and magnitude in the human brain. In:Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Program No.74,6.
    Gregorios-Pippas, L., Tobler, P. N., & Schultz, W. (2009). Short-term temporal discounting of reward value in human ventral striatum. J Neurophysiol.,101,1507-1523.
    Gunturkun, O. (2005). The avian'prefrontal cortex' and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.,15, 686-693.
    Gupta, R., et al. (2009). Declarative memory is critical for sustained advantageous complex decision-making. Neuropsychologia,47,1686-1693.
    Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F.. & Rangel. A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science.324,646-648.
    Hariri, A. R., Brown, S. M., Williamson, D. E., Flory, J. D., de Wit, H., & Manuck, S. B. (2006). Preference for immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity. J. Neurosci.,26,13213-13217.
    Hassani, O. K., Cromwell, H. C., & Schultz, W. (2001). Influence of expectation of different rewards on behavior-related neuronal activity in the striatum. J. Neurophysiol.,85, 2477-2489.
    Hester, R., & Garavan, H. (2004). Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction:evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. J. Neurosci,24,11017-11022.
    Hassani, O. K., Cromwell, H. C., & Schultz, W. (2001). Influence of expectation of different rewards on behavior-related neuronal activity in the striatum. J. Neurophysiol.,85, 2477-2489.
    Hikosaka, K., & Watanabe, M. (2000). Delay activity of orbital and lateral prefrontal neurons of the monkey varying with different rewards. Cereb. Cortex,10,263-271.
    Hikosaka, K., & Watanabe, M. (2004). Long- and short-range reward expectancy in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci.,19,1046-1054.
    Hodgins, D. C., & Engel, A. (2002). Future time perspective in pathological gamblers. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease,190,775-780.
    Hoffman, W. F., Moore, M., Templin, R., McFarland, B., Hitzemann, R. J., & Mitchell, S. H. (2006). Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology,188,162-170.
    Hollerman, J. R., & Schultz, W. (1998). Dopamine neurons report an error in the temporal prediction of reward during learning. Nat. Neuroscl.,1,304-309.
    Holt, D. D., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2003). Is discounting impuisive? Evidence from temporal and probability discounting in gambling and non-gambling college students. Behavioural Processes,64,355-367.
    Hwang, J., Kim, S.,&Lee, D. (2006). Neuronal signals related to delayed reward and its discounted value in the macaque dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In:Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Program No.71.7.
    Isles, A. R., Humby, T., Walters, E., & Wilkinson, L. S. (2004). Common genetic effects on variation in impulsivity and activity in mice. J. Neurosci.24,6733-6740.
    Isles, A. R., Humby, T., & Wilkinson, L. S. (2003). Measuring impulsivity in mice using a novel operant delayed reinforcement task:effects of behavioural manipulations and D-amphetamine. Psychopharmacology (Berlin),170,376-382.
    Ivry, R. B. (1996). The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol,6,851-857.
    Ivry, R. B. (1997). Cerebellar timing systems. Int. Rev. Neurobiol.,41,555-573.
    Ivry, R. B., Spencer, R. M., Zelaznik, H. N., & Diedrichsen, J. (2002). The cerebellum and event timing. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.978,302-317.
    Izawa, E., Aoki, N., & Matsushima, T. (2005). Neural correlates of the proximity and quantity of anticipated food rewards in the ventral striatum of domestic chicks. Eur. J. Neurosci.,22, 1502-1512.
    Izawa, E., Zachar, G., Yanagihara, S., & Matsushima, T. (2003). Localized lesion of caudal part of lobus parolfactorius caused impulsive choice in the domestic chick, evolutionarily conserved function of ventral striatum. J. Neurosci.23,1894-1902.
    Jahanshahi, M., Jones, C. R. G., Dirnberger, G., & Frith, C. D. (2006). The substantia nigra pars compacta and temporal processing. The Journal of Neuroscience,26,12266-12273.
    Janssen, P., & Shadlen, M. N. (2005). A representation of the hazard rate'of elapsed time in macaque area LIP. Nat. Neurosci.,8,234-241.
    Jarvis, E. D., Gunturkun, O., Bruce, L., Csillag, A., Karten, H., Kuenzel, W., et al. (2005). Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.,6,151-159.
    Johnson, E. J., Haubl, G. & Keinan. (2007). Aspects of endowment:a query theory of value construction. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition,33, 461-474.
    Johnson, A., et al. (2007). Integrating hippocampus and striatum in decision-making. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 17,692-697.
    Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2007). The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci.,10,1625-1633.
    Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2009). The neurobiology of decision:consensus and controversy. Neuron,63,733-745.
    Kable, J. W., & Glimcher. P. W. (2010). An "as soon as possible"effect in human intertemporal decision making:behavioral evidence and neural mechanisms. Journal of neurophysiology, 103.2513-2531.
    Kahneman, D., et al. (1982). Judgment under Uncertainty:Heuristics and Biases, Cambridge University Press.
    Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory:an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica,47,263-291.
    Kalenscher, T., & Pennartz, C. M. A. (2008). Is a bird in the hand worth two in the future? The neuroeconomics of intertemporal decision-making. Progress in Neurobiology,84,284-315.
    Kalenscher, T., Diekamp, B., Gunturkun, O. (2003). Neural architecture of choice behaviour in a concurrent interval schedule. Eur. J. Neurosci.,18.2627-2637.
    Kalenscher, T., Windmann, S., Diekamp, B., Rose, J., Gunturkiin, O., & Colombo, M. (2005). Single Units in the Pigeon Brain Integrate Reward Amount and Time-to-Reward in an Impulsive Choice Task. Current Biology,15,594-602.
    Kalenscher, T., Ohmann, T., & Gunturkun, O. (2006a). The neuroscience of impulsive and self-controlled decisions. Int. J. Psychophysiol.,62,203-211.
    Kalenscher, T., Ohmann, T., Windmann, S., Freund, N., & Gunturkun, O. (2006b). Single forebrain neurons represent interval timing and reward amount during response scheduling. Eur. J. Neurosci.,24,2923-2931.
    Kalivas. P. W., & Volkow, N. D. (2005). The neural basis of addiction:A pathology of motivation and choice. Am. J. Psychiatry,162,1403-1413.
    Kheramin, S., Body, S., Mobini, S., Ho, M.Y., Velazquez-Martinez, D.N., Bradshaw, C.M., Szabadi, E., Deakin, J. F., & Anderson, I. M. (2002). Effects of quinolinic acid-induced lesions of the orbital prefrontal cortex on intertemporal choice:a quantitative analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berlin),165,9-11.
    Kim, B. K., & Zauberman, G. (2012). Can Victoria's Secret Change the Future? A Subjective Time Perception Account of Sexual Cue Effects on Impatience. Working paper, Marshall School of Business, University of Southern California.
    Kim, B. K., & Zauberman, G. (2009). Perception of anticipatory time in temporal discounting. Journal ofNeuroscience, Psychology, and Economics,2,91-101.
    Kim, S., Hwang, J., & Lee, D. (2008). Prefrontal coding of temporally discounted values during intertemporal choice. Neuron,59,161-172.
    Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future:Evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General,126,54-70.
    Kirby, K. N., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1995). Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed reward. Psychological Science,6,83-89.
    Kirby, K. N., Marakovic, N. N. (1995). Modeling Myopic Decisions:Evidence for Hyperbolic Delay-Discounting with Subjects and Amounts. Org. Behav. Human Decision Proc.64,22-30.
    Kirby, K. N., & Perry, N. M. (2004). Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction,99,461-471.
    Kirby, K. N., Perry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards then non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 128,78-87.
    Klapproth, F. (2008). Time and decision making in humans. Cognitive, Affective,& Behavioral Neuroscience,8 (4),509-524.
    Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., & Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci.,21, RC159,1-5.
    Kojima, S., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1982). Delay-related activity of prefrontal neurons in rhesus monkeys performing delayed response. Brain Res.,248,43-49.
    Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2003). Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology,5th edition. Worth Publishers, New York.
    Komura, Y., Tamura, R., Uwano, T., Nishijo, H., Kaga, K., & Ono, T. (2001). Retrospective and prospective coding for predicted reward in the sensory thalamus. Nature,412,546-549.
    Lagorio, C. H., Madden, G. J. (2005). Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards Ⅲ: steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards. Behav. Processes,69(2), 173-87.
    Laibson, D. I. (1997). Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112, 443-77.
    Laibson, D. (2001). A cue-theory of consumption. Quarterly Journal of Economics,116,81-119.
    Lane, S.D, Cherek, D.R., Pietras, C. J., Tcheremissine, O. V. (2003). Measurement of delay discounting using trial-by-trial consequences. Behav. Processes,64,287-303.
    Lee, D., Rushworth, M. F. S., Walton, M. E., Watanabe, M., & Sakagami, M. (2007). Functional specialization of the primate frontal cortex during decision making. J. Neurosci.,27, 8170-8173.
    Leland, D. S., & Paulus, M. P. (2005). Increased risk-taking decision-making but not altered response to punishment in stimulant-using young adults. Drug & Alcohol Dependence,78, 83-90.
    Lennings, C.J., & Burns, A. M. (1998). Time perspective:temporal extension, time estimation, and impulsivity. J. Psychol.,132,367-380.
    Leon, M. I., Shadlen, M. N. (1999). Effect of expected reward magnitude on the response of neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque. Neuron,24,415-425.
    Leon, M. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2003). Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron,38,317-327.
    Lewis, P. A., & Miall. R. C. (2003). Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub-and supra-second intervals. Neuropsychologia,41,1583-1592.
    Li, J. Z., Li, S., & Liu, H. (2011). How has the Wenchuan Earthquake influenced people's intertemporal choices? Journal of Applied Social Psychology,41,2739-2752.
    Li. S., Su, Y., & Sun, Y. (2010). The effect of pseudo-immediacy on intertemporal choices. Journal of Risk Research,13,781-787.
    Lissek, S., Gunturkun, O. (2003). Dissociation of extinction and behavioral disinhibition:the role of NMDA receptors in the pigeon associative forebrain during extinction. J. Neurosci.,23, 8119-8124.
    Liu, et al. (2012). The value of emotion:how does episodic prospection modulate delay discounting? Plose One (in press).
    Loewenstein, G. F. (1988). Frames of mind in intertemporal choice. Management Science.34, 200-214.
    Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control:Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,65.272-292.
    Loewenstein, G. (2000). Emotions in Economic Theory and Economic Behavior. American Economic Review Proceedings,90,426-430.
    Loewenstein, G. F., & Prelec, D. (1993). Preferences for sequences of outcomes. Psychological Review,100,91-108.
    Louie. K., & Glimcher, P.W. (2006). Temporal discounting activity in monkey parietal neurons during intertemporal choice. In:Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Program No. 605,5.
    Luhmann, C.C., et al. (2008). Neural dissociation of delay and uncertainty in intertemporal choice. J. Neuroscl. 28,14459-14466.
    Madden, G. J., Raiff, B. R., Lagorio, C. H., Begotka, A.. M.., Mueller, A. M., Hehli, D. J., & Wegener, A. A. (2004). Delay discounting of potentially real and hypothetical rewards:Ⅱ. Between-and within-subject comparisons. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol.,12,251-261.
    Mahajna, A., Benzion, U., Bogaire, R., & Shavit, T. (2008). Subjective discount rates among Israeli Arabs and Israeli Jews. Journal of Socio-Economics,37,2513-2522.
    Malapani, C., Rakitin, B., Levy, R., Meck, W. H., Deweer, B., & Dubois, B. (1998). Coupled temporal memories in Parkinson's disease:A dopamine-related dysfunction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,10,316-331.
    Mariano, T. Y., et al. (2009). Impulsive choice in hippocampal but not orbitofrontal cortex-lesioned rats on a nonspatial decision-making maze task. Eur. J. Neurosci.,30,472-484.
    Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence-detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research,21,139-170.
    Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value Quantitative Analyses of Behavior (Vol.5) (Commons, M.L. et al., eds), In pp.55-73, Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Mazur, J. E. (1984). Tests of an equivalence rule for fixed and varible reinforcer delays. Experimental Psychology:Animal Behavior,10,426-436.
    McClure, S. M., et al. (2003). Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron,38,339-346.
    McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science,306,503-507.
    McClure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience,27,5796-5804.
    McHugh, S. B., Campbell, T. G., Taylor, A. M., Rawlins, J. N. P., & Bannerman, D. M. (2008). A role for dorsal and ventral hippocampus in inter-temporal choice cost-benefit decision making. Behavioral Neuroscience,122,1-8.
    Meek, W. H. (1996). Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.,3,227-242.
    Meek, W. H., & MacDonald, C. J. (2007). Amygdala inactivation reverses fear's ability to impair divided attention and make time stand still. Behavioral Neuroscience,121,707-720.
    Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification:dynamics of willpower. Psychol. Rev.,106,3-19.
    Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 24, 167-202.
    Mischel, M. (1961). Father absence and delay of gratification: Cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 116-124.
    Mischel, M. W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 13-19.
    Mischel, W., & Underwood, B. (1974). Instrumental ideation in delay of gratification. Child Dev., 45, 1083-1088.
    Mischel, W., Shoda, Y, & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933-938.
    Montague, P. R., & Berns, G. S. (2002). Neural economics and the biological substrates of valuation. Neuron, 36, 265-284.
    Mobini, S., Body, S., Ho, M.Y., Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E., Deakin, J. F., & Anderson, I. M. (2002). Effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 160, 290-298.
    Monterosso. J. R., et al. (2007). Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-dependent and comparison subjects performing a delay discounting task. Human Brain Mapping, 28. 383-393.
    Moulton, S.T., & Kosslyn, S.M. (2009). Imagining predictions: mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal B: Society Biological Science, 364, 1273-1280.
    Muraven, M., Tice, D. M. , & Baumeister, R. E. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource:
    Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 774-789.
    Myerson, J., Green, J., Hanson, J. S., Holt, D. D. & Estle, S. J. (2003). Discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards: Processes and traits. Journal of Economic Psychology', 24, 619-635.
    Navarick, D. (2004). Discounting of delayed reinforcers: Measurment by questionaires versus operent choice procedures. Psychol Rec., 54, 85-94.
    O'Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (1999). Doing it now or later. American Economic Review, 89, 103-124.
    Odum, A. L., & Rainaud, C. P. (2003). Discounting of delayed hypothetical money, alcohol, and food. Behavioural Processes, 64, 305-313.
    Ohmura, N. Y., Takahashi, T. & Kitamura, N., (2005). Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarettes. Psychopharmacology, 182, 508-515.
    Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Tanji, K., Suzuki, K., et al. (2003). Thinking of the future and past: The roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. NeuroImage, 19, 1369-1380.
    Olson, E. A., et al. (2007). Adolescents'performance on delay and probability discounting tasks: contributions of age, intelligence, executive functioning, and self-reported externalizing behavior. Pers. Individ. Diff,43,1886-1897.
    Olson, E. A., et al. (2009). White matter integrity predicts delay discounting behavior in 9-to 23-year-olds:a diffusion tensor imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci.21,1406-1421.
    Padoa-Schioppa, C., & Assad, J. A. (2006). Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature,441,223-226.
    Pagnoni, G., et al. (2002). Activity in human ventral striatum locked to errors of reward prediction. Nat. Neurosci.,5.97-98.
    Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology.51,768-774.
    Paulus, M. P. (2007). Decision-making dysfunctions in psychiatry altered homeostatic processing? Science,318,602-606.
    Peters, J. (2011). The role of the medial orbitofrontal cortex in intertemporal choice:prospection or valuation? The Journal of Neuroscience.31,5889-5890.
    Peters, J., & Biichel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron,66,138-148.
    Peters. J., & Buche, C. (2009). Overlapping and distinct neural systems code for subjective value during intertemporal and risky decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience,29. 15727-15734.
    Peters, J., & Biichel, C. (2011). The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,15,227 - 239.
    Petry, N. M. (2001a). Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharnmacology(Berl.),154,243-250.
    Petry, N. M. (2001b). Pathological gamblers, with and without substance use disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates. J. Abnorm. Psychol,110,482-487.
    Pickens, C. L., Saddoris, M. P., Setlow, B., Gallagher, M., Holland, P. C., & Schoenbaum, G. (2003). Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task. J. Neurosci.23,11078-11084.
    Platt, M. L., Glimcher, P. W. (1999). Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature,400,233-238.
    Ploghaus, A., et al. (1999). Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science,284, 1979-1981.
    Ploghaus, A., et al. (2003). Neural circuitry underlying pain modulation:expectation, hypnosis, placebo. Trends Cogn. Sci.,7,197-200.
    Prencipe, A., et al. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. J. Exp. Child Psychol,108,621-637.
    Quintana, J., & Fuster, J. M. (1999). From perception to action:temporal integrative functions of prefrontal and parietal neurons. Cereb. Cortex,9,213-221.
    Rachlin, H. (1995). Self-control:Beyond commitment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,18,109-121.
    Rachlin, H., & Jones, B. A. (2008). Social discounting and delay discounting. Journal of Behavioral decision making,2,29-43.
    Rainer, G., & Miller, E. K. (2002). Time course of object-related neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during a short-term memory task. Eur. J. Neuroscl.,15,1244-1254.
    Rawlins, J. N., et al. (1985). The effects of delaying reward on choice preference in rats with hippocampal or selective septal lesions. Behav. Brain Res.,15,191-203.
    Read, D., Frederick, S., Orsel, B., & Rahman, J. (2005). Four score and Seven Years from Now: The Date/Delay Effect in Temporal Discounting. Management Science,51,1326-1335.
    Read, D., & Roelofsma, P. (2003). Subadditive versus hyperbolic discounting:A comparison of choice and matching. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,91,140-153.
    Reutimann, J., Yakovlev, V., Fusi, S., & Senn, W. (2004). Climbing neuronal activity as an event-based cortical representation of time. J. Neurosci.,24,3295-3303.
    Reynolds, B. (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans:relations to drug use and gambling. Behav Pharmacol.,17,651-667.
    Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. (2004). Measuring state changes in human delay discounting:an experiential discounting task. Behav. Processes,67,343-356.
    Roelfsema, P.R. (2002). Do neurons predict the future? Science.295,227.
    Roesch, M. R., & Olson, C. R. (2004). Neuronal activity related to reward value and motivation in primate frontal cortex. Science,304,307-310.
    Roesch, M. R., & Olson, C. R. (2005a). Neuronal activity in primate orbitofrontal cortex reflects the value of time. Journal of neurophysiology,94,2457-2471.
    Roesch, M. R., & Olson, C. R., (2005b). Neuronal activity dependent on anticipated and elapsed delay in macaque prefrontal cortex, frontal and supplementary eye fields, and premotor cortex. Journal of neurophysiology,94,1469-1497.
    Roesch, M. R., Taylor, A. R., & Schoenbaum, G. (2006). Encoding of time-discounted rewards in orbitofrontal cortex is independent of value representation. Neuron,51,509-520.
    Roesch, M. R., Takahashi, Y., Gugsa, N., Bissonette, G. B., Schoenbaum, G. (2007). Previous cocaine exposure makes rats hypersensitive to both delay and reward magnitude. J. Neurosci., 27,245-250.
    Rosati, A. G., Stevens, J. R., Hare, B., & Hauser, M. D. (2007). The evolutionary origins of human patience:Temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Current Biology 17,1663-1668.
    Rudebeck, P. H., Walton, M. E., Smyth, A. N., Bannerman, D. M., & Rushworth, M. F. (2006). Separate neural pathways process different decision costs. Nat Neurosci.,9,1161-1168.
    Saddoris, M. P., Gallagher, M., & Schoenbaum, G. (2005). Rapid associative encoding in basolateral amygdala depends on connections with orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron,46,321-331.
    Sanfey, A. G., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., Cohen, J. D. (2006). Neuroeconomics: cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends Cogn. Sci.,10,108-116.
    Sakurai, Y., Takahashi, S., & Inoue, M. (2004). Stimulus duration in working memory is represented by neuronal activity in the monkey prefrontal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci.20, 1069-1080.
    Samuelson, P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. The Review of Economic Studies,4, 155-161.
    Sato, T. R., & Schall, J. D. (2003). Effects of stimulus-response compatibility on neural selection in frontal eye field. Neuron,38,637-648.
    Sayette, M. A., Loewenstein, G., Kirchner, T. R., & Travis, T. (2005). Effects of smoking urge on temporal cognition. Psychology of Addictive Behavior,19,88-93.
    Schacter, D.L., & Addis, D.R. (2009). On the nature of medial temporal lobe contributions to the constructive simulation of future events. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal B:Society Biological Science.364,1245-1253.
    Schacter. D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.,8,657-661.
    Scheres, A., et al. (2006). Temporal and probabilistic discounting of rewards in children and adolescents:effects of age and ADHD symptoms. Neuropsychologia,44,2092-2103.
    Schoenbaum, G., Chiba, A. A., Gallagher, M. (1998). Orbitofrontal Cortex and basolateral amygdala encode expected outcomes during learning. Nat. Neurosci.,1,155-159.
    Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron,36,241-263.
    Schultz, W. (2004). Neural coding of basic reward terms of animal learning theory, game theory,
    microeconomics and behavioural ecology. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.,14,139-147.
    Schweighofer, N, Bertin, M, Shishida, K, Okamoto, Y, Tanaka. S. C, Yamawaki, S, & Doya, K. (2008). Low-serotonin levels increase delayed reward discounting in humans. J. Neurosci.,28, 4528-4532.
    Schweighofer, N., Shishida, K., Han, C.E., Okamoto, Y., Tanaka, S.C., Yamawaki, S., & Doya, K., (2006). Humans can adopt optimal discounting strategy under real-time constraints. PLoS Comput. Biol.,2,1349-1356.
    Sellitto, M., Ciaramelli, E., & di, Pellegrino, G, (2010). Myopic discounting of future rewards after medial orbitofrontal damage in humans. J. Neurosci.,30,16429-16436.
    Shamosh, N. A., et al. (2008). Individual differences in delay discounting:relation to intelligence, working memory, and anterior prefrontal cortex. Psychol. Sci.,19,904-911.
    Shamosh, N. A., & Gray, J. R. (2008). Delay discounting and intelligence:A meta-analysis. Intelligence,38,289-305.
    Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature,450,102-105.
    Shuler, M. G., & Bear, M. F. (2006). Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. Science,311, 1606-1609.
    Simons, J., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Lacante, M. (2004). Placing motivation and future time perspective theory in a temporal perspective. Educational Psychology Review,16,121-139.
    Soman, D., Ainslie, G., Frederick, S., Li, X. P., Lynch, J., Moreau, P., et al. (2005). The psychology of intertemporal discounting:Why are distant Events valued differently from proximal ones? Marketing Letters,16,347-360.
    Somerville, L. H., Hare, T., & Casey, B. J. (2011). Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,23, 2123-2134.
    Staddon, J. E. R., & Cerutti, D. T. (2003). Operant behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol.,54,115-144.
    Steinberg, L., et al. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Dev., 80.28-44.
    Stephens, D. W., & Anderson, D. (2001). The adaptive value of preference for mmediacy:when shortsighted rules have farsighted consequences. Behav. Ecol.12,330-339.
    Stephens, D. W., & Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging Theory. Monographs in Behavior and Ecology. Princeton University Press, Princeton, N. J.
    Stevens. J. R., Hallinan, E. V., Hauser, M. D. (2005). The ecology and evolution of patience in two New World monkeys. Biol. Lett.1,223-226.
    Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., & Edwards, C. S. (1994). The consideration of future consequences:Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,66,742-752.
    Sugrue, L. P., Corrado, G. S., & Newsome, W. T. (2004). Matching behavior and the representation of value in the parietal cortex., Science,304,1782-1787.
    Szpunar, K. K., Chan, J. C. K., & McDermott, K. B. (2009). Contextual processing in episodic future thought. Cerebral Cortex,19,1539-1548.
    Szpunar, K. K., Watson, J. M., & McDermott, K. B. (2007). Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA,104,642-647.
    Takahashi, T. (2004). Cortisol levels and time-discounting of monetary gain in humans. Neuroreport,15,2145-2147.
    Takahashi, T. (2007). Hyperbolic discounting may be reduced to electrical coupling in dopaminergic neural circuits. Medical Hypotheses,69,195-198.
    Takahashi, T. (2005). Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be attributable to logarithmic time-perception. Med. Hypotheses,65,691-693.
    Takahashi, T. (2006). Time-estimation error following Weber-Fechner law may explain subadditive time-discounting. Medical Hypotheses,67,1372-1374.
    Takahashi, T., et al. (2008). Psychophysics of time perception and intertemporal choice models. Phvs.A,387,2066-2074.
    Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality,72, 271-322.
    Thaler, R. (1981). Some empirical evidence of dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters,8, 201-207.
    Tobler, P. N., Fiorillo, C. D., & Schultz, W. (2005). Adaptive coding of reward value by dopamine neurons. Science,307,1642-1645.
    Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review,110,403-421.
    Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-Level Theory of Psychological Distance. Psychological Review.117,440-463.
    Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Wakslak, C. (2007). Construal levels and psychological distance: Effects on representation, prediction, evaluation, and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology,17,83-95.
    Twenge, J. M., et al. (2003). Social exclusion and the deconstructed state:Time perception, meaninglessness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol,85, 409-423.
    Van, den, Bergh, B., Dewitte, S., & Warlop, L. (2008). Bikinis Instigate Generalized Impatience in Intertemporal Choice. Journal of Consumer Research,35,85-97.
    Van Duuren, E.. Nieto-Esca'mez, F. A.. Joosten, R. N. J. M. A., Visser, R., Mulder, A. B., Pennartz, C. M. A. (2007). Neural coding of actual and expected reward magnitude in the orbitofrontal cortex of the rat. Learn. Mem.14,613-621.
    Voorn, P., Vanderschuuren, L. J., Groenewegen, H. J., Robbins, T. W., & Pennartz, C. M. A. (2004). Putting a spin on the dorsal-ventral divide of the striatum. Trends in Neurosciences,27, 468-474.
    Vuchinich, R. E., & Simpson, C. A. (1998). Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol.,6,292-305.
    Wager, T. D., et al. (2004). Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science.303,1162-1167.
    Wang. X. T., & Dvorak, R. D. (2010). Sweet future:fluctuating blood glucose levels affect future discounting. Psychol. Sci.,21.183-188.
    Wallis, J. D., & Miller, E. K. (2003). Neuronal activity in primate dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal cortex during performance of a reward preference task. European Journal ofNeuroscience,18. 2069-2081.
    Watanabe, M. (1996). Reward expectancy in primate prefrontal neurons. Nature,382,629-632.
    Watt, J. D. (1991). Effect of boredom proneness on time perception. Psychol. Rep.,69.323■327.
    Wearden, J.H., & Penton-Voak, I.S. (1995). Feeling the heat:body temperature and the rate of subjective time, revisited. O. J. Exp. Psychol. B.48,129-141.
    Weber, B. J., & Huettel, S. A. (2008). The neural substrates of probabilistic and intertemporal decision making. Brain Res.,1234,104-115.
    Weber, E. U., Johnson, E. J., Milch. K. F., Chang, H., Brodscholl, J. C., & Goldstein, D. G. (2007). Asymmetric discounting in intertemporal choice:a query theory account. Psychological Science,18(6),516-523.
    Wilhelm, C. J., & Mitchell, S. H. (2009). Strain differences in delay discounting using inbred rats. Genes Brain Behav..8,426-434.
    Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2004). Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci.,271,177-179.
    Winstanley, C. A., Baunez, C., Theobald, D. E., & Robbins, T. W. (2005). Lesions to the subthalamic nucleus decrease impulsive choice but impair autoshaping in rats:the importance of the basal ganglia in Pavlovian conditioning and impulse control. Eur. J. Neurosci.,21, 3107-3116.
    Winstanley, C. A., Theobald, D. E., Cardinal, R. N., Robbins, T. W. (2004). Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. J. Neurosci., 24, 4718-4722.
    Winstanley, C. A., Theobald, D. E., Dalley, J. W., Cardinal, R. N., Robbins, T. W. (2006). Double dissociation between serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during a test of impulsive choice. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 106-114.
    Wittmann, M., Leland, D. S. & Paulus, M. P. (2007). Time and decision making: differential contribution of the posterior insular cortex and the striatum during a delay discounting task. Experimental Brain Research, 179, 643-653.
    Wittmann, M., Leland, D. S., Churan, J., & Paulus, M. P. (2007). Impaired time perception and motor timing in stimulant-dependent subjects. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 90, 183-192.
    Wittmann, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 7-12.
    Wittmann, M., et al. (2006). The relation between the experience of time and psychological distress in patients with hematological malignancies. Palliat. Support. Care, 4, 357-363.
    Yang, T, & Shadlen, M. N. (2007). Probabilistic reasoning by neurons. Nature, 447, 1075-1080.
    Yi, R., Gatchalian, K. M., & Bickel, W. K. (2006). Discounting of past outcomes. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 14, 311-317.
    Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci., 6, 12-16.
    Zauberman, G., Kim, B. K, Malkoc, S. A., & Bettman, J. R. (2009). Discounting time and time discounting: subjective time perception and intertemporal preferences. Journal of Marketing Research, XLVI, 543-556.
    Zimbardo, P. G., Keough, K. A., & Boyd, J. N. (1997). Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 1007-1023.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700