内隐自杀意念测量及在自杀逃避理论中的应用
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
自杀是导致个体死亡的主要原因之一,也是一种复杂的人类行为。根据2012年世界卫生组织的报告,每年有近一百万人死于自杀,自杀死亡的人数远大于被谋杀的人数,与45年前相比自杀率上升了60%。许多研究者认为,自杀意念是自杀前的重要指标。因此通过对自杀意念的研究,可以更好地理解、预防和干预自杀行为。然而,由于人们有意、无意地回避报告个人自杀意念的存在,导致自杀意念的客观测量非常困难,对于自杀预防和干预也难以实施。然而,由于内隐认知测量规避了心理测验中社会赞许性的弊端,可能有助于自杀风险的评估。
     目前有20余种内隐认知测验的方法。其中,内隐联想测验应用最广、效果最明显,并已积累了大量研究证据;在心理健康、消费决策、选举行为等方面的评估中,内隐联想测验被用于众多心理结构的测量,如自尊、态度、偏见和自我概念等。于是,我们想探究那些有自杀意念并尝试过自杀的个体,是否将“自我”与“死亡”连接得更加紧密,即具有内隐自杀意念;探究内隐自杀意念对自杀行为的预测是否比自我报告法更好;并采用ERPs技术探讨无自杀意念者内隐联想测验的认知加工机制。
     逃避理论对自杀行为的发生进行了理论解释,在这个理论中自杀涉及到六个阶段。第一个阶段是由于没有达到期望目标产生的失败感或失望感。这可能是由于期望太高导致的,也可能是由于现实太坏导致的,也可能是两方面的原因同时作用的结果,本质是个体的现实和期望间存在差距。第二个阶段是将失败归咎于自己产生的自责。第三至第六阶段分别为:自我意识变化、负性情绪、认知解构和最终导致自杀意念的产生。本研究试图通过探究逃避理论与内隐自杀意念产生之间的关系,验证逃避理论能否解释内隐自杀意念在自杀形成中的作用。
     本研究的主要研究结果有:
     (1)那些曾经有过自杀行为的个体在内隐联想测验中将“自我”与“死亡”进行了正向的内隐连接(M=0.34,SE=0.1),而“自我”与“死亡”的内隐连接在仅有自杀意念的被试中较弱(M=-0.06,SE=0.06),而那些没有自杀意念和自杀行为的个体在内隐联想测验中将“自我”与“死亡”进行了负向的内隐连接(M=-0.34,SE=0.07)。通过方差分析发现,三组被试间在内隐联想测验中的表现具有显著的差异,F(2,69)=17.89,p <0.01。研究数据说明内隐联想测验测量自杀意念有效性较好。
     (2)根据自杀的逃避理论建立的模型经过修改后,与本研究的数据具有较好的拟合度,卡方为17.54,自由度为14,p=0.23,RMSEA=0.04,GFI=0.98。说明自杀逃避理论可以用于解释内隐自杀意念的产生和发展。
     (3)在启动失败认知、启动成功认知和控制组三种实验条件下,被试的内隐自杀意念强度不同,F(2,135)=13.42,p <.001,η2=.17。另外,个体的控制点倾向会影响到该效应,F(2,132)=13.38,p <.001,η2=.16。具体地,在内控的个体中该启动效应依然存在,F(2,67)=15.46,p <.001,η2=.32,而在外控的个体中该效应却消失了,F(2,65)=1.57,p=.22。这些研究数据符合根据自杀逃避理论建立的假设,即失败会导致自杀意念增加而成功会导致自杀意念减少,并且该效应会受到个体控制点倾向的影响,在内控倾向的个体中启动效应会增大,而在外控倾向的个体中启动效应会减小或消失。
     (4)在内隐联想测验中,不相容任务组的反应时(M=621.90ms,SD=64ms),显著长于相容任务组的反应时(M=566.38ms,SD=60ms),t(20)=3.31,p <0.01。是否相容与是否任务转换对反应时间的效应存在交互作用,F(1,20)=4.43,p <0.05。在相容条件下,任务是否转换对反应时间没有显著的影响。在不相容条件下,任务转换组的反应时(M=654.93ms,SD=71.61ms)显著长于非任务转换组的反应时(M=575.21ms,SD=79.53ms)。研究还发现相容条件下刺激诱导的P300成分的波幅(M=4.86μV,SD=1.84μV)大于不相容条件下刺激诱导的P300成分的波幅(M=3.51μV,SD=1.55μV),t(20)=4.44,p <0.01。在不相容任务下,需要任务转换的刺激诱导的P300成分的波幅(M=3.96μV,SD=1.55μV)与不需要任务转换的刺激诱导的P300成分的波幅(M=3.17μV,SD=1.75μV)不一致,t(20)=2.58,p <0.05。这些研究结果反应出在内隐联想测验中,相容任务需要较少的认知资源,个体有更多的认知资源进行刺激加工,使得个体在反应中具有更高的自信;在不相容任务中,对于那些不需要任务转换的刺激,被试主观上认为其发生的概率较高会影响到其对刺激的认知加工
Suicide is a leading cause of death worldwide and is among the most perplexing ofall human behaviors. According to the World Health Organization (2012), almost onemillion people die by suicide every year. Suicide accounts for more than all deaths frommurder. In the past45years, suicide rates have increased by60%worldwide. Manyresearchers consider that suicidal thought is the primary antecedent of suicidal behaviors.Many researchers thus believe that by understanding the origin of suicidal thought, we canbetter understand, predict, and treat suicidal behavior. However, suicide is difficult topredict and prevent because people who consider killing themselves often are unwilling orunable to report their intentions. Advances in the measurement of implicit cognition mayprovide a behavioral marker for suicide risk.
     There are more than20measurement procedures in the measurement of implicit cognition. Among these, the Implicit Association Test (IAT) provides the bulk ofaccumulated research evidence. Furthermore, IAT has been adapted to measure a widevariety of mental contents including stereotypes, attitudes, self-esteem, and self-concepts,across a wide variety of topics such as mental health, consumer decision making, andpolitical behavior. We tested whether individuals who have made a decision to killthemselves would reveal stronger implicit cognition associating self with death andwhether the strength of such an association would predict suicide attempts beyond thatachieved with suicidal thoughts from self-report. And study of the cognitive processesinvolved in the IAT is necessary to establish the validity of this measure and to suggestfurther refinements to its use and interpretation. The current study used ERPs to studycognitive processes associated with the IAT.
     Escape theory is an integrative framework proposing a causal chain that leads tosuicide. In this framework, suicide involves an orderly progression through six distinctstages. The initial step is a major failure or disappointment that is due to events fallingseverely short of standards or expectations. This may occur because events are verynegative, because standards are too high, or both. It is the discrepancy between the realityand standard that is crucial. The second stage involves self-blame: Responsibility forfailure is attributed to the self. The following steps successively involve highself-awareness, negative affect and feelings, a state of cognitive deconstruction, andfinally several consequences of cognitive deconstruction that may increase the appeal ofsuicide. The other purpose of the present study was to assess the effectiveness of theEscape Theory of Suicide in accounting for the development of implicit cognitionassociating self with death.
     Through the above study, the mainly results were found as following:
     (1) Nonsuicidal individuals show a negative association between death and oneself(M=-0.34, SE=0.07); suicide ideators show a small negative association between deathand oneself (M=-0.06, SE=0.06); and suicide attempters show a large positiveassociation between s death and oneself (M=0.34, SE=0.1). A one-way ANOVArevealed a significant group effect, F (2,69)=17.89, p <0.01. These results support the validity of implicit association test as a measure of suicide thoughts.
     (2) The final model from the Escape Theory of Suicide fit our data very well. Thechi-square for this model was17.54, df=14, p=0.23, RMSEA=0.04, and GFI=0.98.Our findings confirm the predictive validity of the Escape Theory of Suicide.
     (3) Results revealed significant differences between groups (failure-related priminggroup, success-related priming group, and control group) in accessibility to implicitsuicidal mind, F (2,135)=13.42, p <.001, η2=.17. Furthermore, priming manipulationinteracted with individual differences in locus of control (LOC), F (2,132)=13.38,p <.001, η2=.16. Significant differences in accessibility to implicit suicidal mind wereobserved in individuals with internal LOC, F (2,67)=15.46, p <.001, η2=.32, whileeffects of priming manipulation were eliminated in individuals with external LOC, F (2,65)=1.57, p=.22. The results of our experiment were in line with our predictions thatfailure increases implicit suicidal mind and success decreases implicit suicidal mind. Thisstudy supports our hypothesis that the impact of failure or success priming on associationsbetween self and death is more pronounced among individuals with internal LOC, andabsent among individuals with external LOC.
     (4) The fourth study found response time was significantly longer in the incongruentcondition (M=621.90ms, SD=64ms) than in the congruent condition (M=566.38ms,SD=60ms), t(20)=3.31, p <0.01. A two-way interaction between condition and task setswitching was found for response time, F (1,20)=4.43, p <0.05. Task switching had noeffect in the congruent condition. Task switching did however significantly prolongresponse time in the Incongruent condition (for task switch: M=654.93ms, SD=71.61ms; for no task switch: M=575.21ms, SD=79.53ms). And the study also foundsignificant differences in P300amplitude in the congruent (M=4.86μV, SD=1.84μV)and incongruent conditions (M=3.51μV, SD=1.55μV), t (20)=4.44, p <0.01. Therewere differences in P300amplitude between trials of the incongruent condition thatrequired (M=3.96μV, SD=1.55μV) or did not require a task switch (M=3.17μV, SD=1.75μV), t (20)=2.58, p <0.05. The less demanding nature of the congruent conditionmay have resulted in greater resources being available to process the stimuli, resulting in less uncertainty about the correct response to the stimulus. In the incongruent condition,there is greater perceived local probability on no switch trials compared to switch trials.
引文
[1] Phillips MR, Li X, Zhang Y. Suicide rates in China,1995–99. The Lancet.2002,359(9309):835-840.
    [2]李献云,费立鹏,张亚利,徐东,童永胜,杨甫德,况利. Beck自杀意念量表中文版在大学学生中应用的信效度.中国心理卫生杂志.2011,25(11):862-866.
    [3]王黎明,申彦丽,梁执群,罗志懿,张克让. Beck自杀意念量表中文版评价抑郁症患者的信、效度.中国健康心理学杂志.2012,20(01):159-160.
    [4]王学志,宫火良,康晓然,刘雯雯,董晓静,马彦锋.青少年自杀意念量表中文版在高中生应用的信效度分析.中国健康心理学杂志.2011,19(08):964-966.
    [5]张康莉,张晓明,辛阔林,于莉莉.自编军人自杀倾向量表的信效度检验.解放军预防医学杂志.2010,28(01):27-30.
    [6] Wenzel A, Brown GK, Beck AT. Cognitive therapy for suicidal patients: Scientific andclinical applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association,2009.
    [7] O'Conno RC. Suicidal behavior as a cry of pain: Test of a psychological model.Archives of Suicide Research.2003,7(4):297-308.
    [8] Wenzel A, Beck AT. A cognitive model of suicidal behavior: Theory and treatment.Applied and Preventive Psychology.2008,12(4):189-201.
    [9] Nock MK, Banaji MR. Assessment of self-injurious thoughts using a behavioral test.The American journal of psychiatry.2007,164(5):820-823.
    [10] Wilson TD. Know Thyself. Perspectives on Psychological Science.2009,4(4):384-389
    [11] Busch KA, Fawcett J, Jacobs DG. Clinical correlates of inpatient suicide. The Journalof clinical psychiatry.2003,64(1):14-19.
    [12] De Houwer J, Teige-Mocigemba S, Spruyt A, Moors A. Implicit measures: Anormative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin.2009,135(3):347-368.
    [13]涂尔干,冯韵文.自杀论:社会学研究.北京:商务印书馆,1996.
    [14] Baechler J, Cooper B. Suicides. New York: Basic Books,1979.
    [15] Farberow N, Schneidman E. Cry for help. New York: McGraw-Hill,1961.
    [16] Gelder M, Gath D, Mayou R. Oxford textbook of psychiatry. London: OxfordUniversity Press,1989.
    [17] Dorpat TL, Boswell JW. An evaluation of suicidal intent in suicide attempts.Comprehensive Psychiatry.1963,4(2):117-125.
    [18] Nock MK, Borges G, Bromet EJ, Alonso J, Angermeyer M, Beautrais A, Bruffaerts R,Chiu W, De Girolamo G, Gluzman S. Cross-national prevalence and risk factors forsuicidal ideation, plans and attempts. The British Journal of Psychiatry.2008,192(2):98-105.
    [19] Desjeux G, Labarère J, Galoisy-Guibal L, Ecochard R. Suicide in the French armedforces. European journal of epidemiology.2004,19(9):823-829.
    [20] Mahon MJ, Tobin JP, Cusack DA, Kelleher C, Malone KM. Suicide amongregular-duty military personnel: a retrospective case-control study ofoccupation-specific risk factors for workplace suicide. American Journal ofPsychiatry.2005,162(9):1688-1696.
    [21] Selby EA, Anestis MD, Bender TW, Ribeiro JD, Nock MK, Rudd MD, Bryan CJ,Lime IC, Baker MT, Gutierrez PM, Joiner Jr TE. Overcoming the fear of lethal injury:Evaluating suicidal behavior in the military through the lens of theInterpersonal–Psychological Theory of Suicide. Clinical Psychology Review.2010,30:298–307.
    [22] Helmkamp JC. Suicides in the military:1980–1992. Military Medicine.1995,160(2):45-50.
    [23] Cavanagh J, Carson A, Sharpe M, Lawrie S. Psychological autopsy studies of suicide:a systematic review. Psychological medicine.2003,33(3):395-405.
    [24] Ernst C, Lalovic A, Lesage A, Seguin M, Tousignant M, Turecki G. Suicide and noaxis I psychopathology. BMC psychiatry.2004,4(1):7.
    [25] Bostwick JM, Pankratz VS. Affective disorders and suicide risk: a reexamination.American Journal of Psychiatry.2000,157(12):1925-1932.
    [26] Herzog DB, Greenwood DN, Dorer DJ, Flores AT, Ekeblad ER, Richards A, BlaisMA, Keller MB. Mortality in eating disorders: a descriptive study. InternationalJournal of Eating Disorders.2000,28(1):20-26.
    [27] Harris EC, Barraclough B. Suicide as an outcome for mental disorders. Ameta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry.1997,170(3):205-228.
    [28] Palmer BA, Pankratz VS, Bostwick JM. The lifetime risk of suicide in schizophrenia:a reexamination. Archives of General Psychiatry.2005,62(3):247-253.
    [29] Joiner Jr TE, Van Orden KA. The interpersonal-psychological theory of suicidalbehavior indicates specific and crucial psychotherapeutic targets. InternationalJournal of Cognitive Therapy.2008,1(1):80-89.
    [30] Suominen K, Isomets E, Suokas J, Haukka J, Achte K, L nnqvist J. Completedsuicide after a suicide attempt: a37-year follow-up study. American Journal ofPsychiatry.2004,161(3):562-563.
    [31] Whitlock F. Suicide and physical illness. Baltimore: Williams&Wilkins,1986.
    [32] Chochinov HM, Hack T, Hassard T, Kristjanson LJ, McClement S, Harlos M. Dignitytherapy: a novel psychotherapeutic intervention for patients near the end of life.Journal of Clinical Oncology.2005,23(24):5520-5525.
    [33] Beck AT, Brown G, Berchick RJ, Stewart BL, Steer RA. Relationship betweenhopelessness and ultimate suicide: A replication with psychiatric outpatients. Focus.2006,4(2):291.
    [34] Baumeister RF. Suicide as escape from self. Psychological Review.1990,97(1):90-113.
    [35] Higgins ET. Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review.1987,94(3):319-340.
    [36] Dean PJ, Range LM, Goggin WC. The Escape Theory of Suicide in college students:Testing a model that includes perfectionism. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior.1996,26(2):181-186.
    [37] Chatard A, Selimbegovi L. When self-destructive thoughts flash through the mind:Failure to meet standards affects the accessibility of suicide-related thoughts. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology.2011,100(4):587-605.
    [38] Duval S, Hensley V. Extensions of objective self-awareness theory: The focus ofattention-causal attribution hypothesis. New directions in attribution research.1976,1:165-198.
    [39] Kruglanski AW, Shah JY, Fishbach A, Friedman R, Chun WY, Sleeth-Keppler D. Atheory of goal systems. Advances in experimental social psychology.2002,34:331-378.
    [40] Joiner T. Why people die by suicide. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press,2005.
    [41] Joiner TE, Pettit JW, Walker RL, Voelz ZR, Cruz J, Rudd MD, Lester D. Perceivedburdensomeness and suicidality: Two studies on the suicide notes of those attemptingand those completing suicide. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology.2002,21(5):531-545.
    [42] Holm-Denoma JM, Witte TK, Gordon KH, Herzog DB, Franko DL, Fichter M,Quadflieg N, Joiner Jr TE. Deaths by suicide among individuals with anorexia asarbiters between competing explanations of the anorexia–suicide link. Journal ofaffective disorders.2008,107(1):231-236.
    [43] Linehan MM, Goodstein JL, Nielsen SL, Chiles JA. Reasons for staying alive whenyou are thinking of killing yourself: the reasons for living inventory. Journal ofConsulting and Clinical Psychology.1983,51(2):276-286.
    [44] Orbach I, Mikulincer M, King R, Cohen D, Stein D. Thresholds and tolerance ofphysical pain in suicidal and nonsuicidal adolescents. Journal of Consulting andClinical Psychology.1997,65(4):646-652.
    [45] Orbach I, Palgi Y, Stein D, Har-Even D, Lotem-peleg M, Asherov J, Elizur A.Tolerance for physical pain in suicidal subjects. Death studies.1996,20(4):327-341.
    [46] Orbach I, Stein D, Palgi Y, Asherov J, Har-Even D, Elizur A. Perception of physicalpain in accident and suicide attempt patients: self-preservation vs self-destruction.Journal of psychiatric research.1996,30(4):307-320.
    [47] Rudd MD, Joiner T, Rajad MH. Relationships among suicide ideators, attempters, andmultiple attempters in a young-adult sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.1996,105(4):541.
    [48] Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporaldynamics of affect. Psychological Review.1974,81(2):119.
    [49] Henry AF, Short JF. Suicide and homicide: some economic, sociological andpsychological aspects of aggression. New York: Free Press,1954.
    [50] Braswell H, Kushner HI. Suicide, social integration, and masculinity in the U.S.military. Social Science and Medicine.2012,74(4):530-536.
    [51] Douglas JD. The social meanings of suicide. Princeton: Princeton University Press,1967.
    [52] Kang HK, Bullman TA. Risk of suicide among US veterans after returning from theIraq or Afghanistan war zones. JAMA: the journal of the American MedicalAssociation.2008,300(6):652-653.
    [53] Adams DP, Barton C, Mitchell GL, Moore AL, Einagel V. Hearts and minds: Suicideamong United States combat troops in Vietnam,1957–1973. Social Science&Medicine.1998,47(11):1687-1694.
    [54] Bullman TA, Kang HK. The risk of suicide among wounded Vietnam veterans.American Journal of Public Health.1996,86(5):662-667.
    [55] Nademin E, Jobes DA, Pflanz SE, Jacoby AM, Ghahramanlou-Holloway M, CampiseR, Joiner T, Wagner BM, Johnson L. An investigation of interpersonal-psychologicalvariables in Air Force suicides: A controlled-comparison study. Archives of SuicideResearch.2008,12(4):309-326.
    [56] Kaplan MS, Huguet N, McFarland BH, Newsom JT. Suicide among male veterans: aprospective population-based study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health.2007,61(7):619-624.
    [57] Jones B. Overuse injuries of the lower extremities associated with marching, joggingand running: A review. Military Medicine.1983,148:783-787.
    [58] Scoville SL, Gardner JW, Potter RN. Traumatic deaths during US Armed Forces basictraining,1977-2001. American journal of preventive medicine.2004,26(3):194-204.
    [59] Thoresen S, Mehlum L, Rysamb E, Tnnessen A. Risk factors for completed suicide inveterans of peacekeeping: repatriation, negative life events, and marital status.Archives of Suicide Research.2006,10(4):353-363.
    [60] Orsillo SM, Roemer L, Litz BT, Ehlich P, Friedman MJ. Psychiatric symptomatologyassociated with contemporary peacekeeping: an examination of post-missionfunctioning among peacekeepers in Somalia. Journal of Traumatic Stress.1998,11(4):611-625.
    [61] Elder GH, Clipp EC. Combat experience and emotional health: Impairment andresilience in later life. Journal of personality.2006,57(2):311-341.
    [62] Vogt DS, King DW, King LA, Savarese VW, Suvak MK. War-Zone Exposure andLong-Term General Life Adjustment Among Vietnam Veterans: Findings From TwoPerspectives. Journal of Applied Social Psychology.2004,34(9):1797-1824.
    [63] Brenner LA, Gutierrez PM, Cornette MM, Betthauser LM, Bahraini N, Staves PJ. Aqualitative study of potential suicide risk factors in returning combat veterans.Journal of Mental Health Counseling.2008,30(3):211-225.
    [64] Ponteva M, Jormanainen V, Nurro S, Lehesjoki M. Mortality after the UN service.Follow-up study of the Finnish peace-keeping contingents in the years1969-1996.International Review of the Armed Forces Medical Services.2000,73(4):235-239.
    [65] Hyer L, McCranie EW, Woods MG, Boudewyns PA. Suicidal behavior among chronicVietnam theatre veterans with PTSD. Journal of clinical psychology.1990,46(6):713-721.
    [66] Nosek BA, Hawkins CB, Frazier RS. Implicit social cognition from measures tomechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.2011,15(4):152-159.
    [67] Payne BK, Cheng CM, Govorun O, Stewart BD. An inkblot for attitudes: affectmisattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.2005,89(3):277.
    [68] Nosek BA, Smyth FL, Hansen JJ, Devos T, Lindner NM, Ranganath KA, Smith CT,Olson KR, Chugh D, Greenwald AG. Pervasiveness and correlates of implicitattitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology.2007,18(1):36-88.
    [69] Greenwald AG, Poehlman TA, Uhlmann EL, Banaji MR. Understanding and usingthe Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology.2009,97(1):17-41.
    [70] Greenwald AG, Smith CT, Sriram N, Bar‐Anan Y, Nosek BA. Implicit race attitudespredicted vote in the2008US presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues andPublic Policy.2009,9(1):241-253.
    [71] Nosek BA, Smyth FL, Sriram N, Lindner NM, Devos T, Ayala A, Bar-Anan Y, BerghR, Cai H, Gonsalkorale K. National differences in gender–science stereotypes predictnational sex differences in science and math achievement. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Sciences.2009,106(26):10593-10597.
    [72] Rooth D-O. Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: Real world evidence.Labour Economics.2010,17(3):523-534.
    [73] Friese M, Hofmann W, W nke M. When impulses take over: Moderated predictivevalidity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice andconsumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology.2008,47(3):397-419.
    [74] Hofmann W, Friese M, Roefs A. Three ways to resist temptation: The independentcontributions of executive attention, inhibitory control, and affect regulation to theimpulse control of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.2009,45(2):431-435.
    [75] Galdi S, Arcuri L, Gawronski B. Automatic mental associations predict future choicesof undecided decision-makers. Science.2008,321(5892):1100-1102.
    [76] Smith ER, DeCoster J. Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology:Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality andsocial psychology review.2000,4(2):108-131.
    [77] Bargh JA, Ferguson MJ. Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mentalprocesses. Psychological bulletin.2000,126(6):925.
    [78] Conrey FR, Sherman JW, Gawronski B, Hugenberg K, Groom CJ. Separatingmultiple processes in implicit social cognition: the quad model of implicit taskperformance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.2005,89(4):469-487.
    [79] Gonsalkorale K, Sherman JW, Klauer KC. Aging and prejudice: Diminishedregulation of automatic race bias among older adults. Journal of Experimental SocialPsychology.2009,45(2):410-414.
    [80]罗耀嘉.认知神经科学教程.北京:北京大学出版社,2006.
    [81]魏景汉,罗跃嘉.认知事件相关脑电位教程.北京:经济日报出版社,2002.
    [82] Hillyard SA, Kutas M. Electrophysiology of cognitive processing. Annual review ofpsychology.1983,34(1):33-61.
    [83] Rugg MD, Doyle MC, Wells T. Word and nonword repetition within-andacross-modality: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.1995,7(2):209-227.
    [84] Bentin S, Kutas M, Hillyard SA. Semantic processing and memory for attended andunattended words in dichotic listening: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.1995,21(1):54.
    [85] Cauquil AS, Edmonds GE, Taylor MJ. Is the face-sensitive N170the only ERP notaffected by selective attention? Neuroreport.2000,11(10):2167-2171.
    [86] Rossion B, Gauthier I, Goffaux V, Tarr M, Crommelinck M. Expertise training withnovel objects leads to left-lateralized facelike electrophysiological responses.Psychological Science.2002,13(3):250-257.
    [87] Pritchard WS. Psychophysiology of P300. Psychological Bulletin; PsychologicalBulletin.1981,89(3):506.
    [88] N t nen R, Gaillard AW, M ntysalo S. Early selective-attention effect on evokedpotential reinterpreted. Acta psychologica.1978,42(4):313-329.
    [89] Coombs DW, Miller HL, Alarcon R, Herlihy C, Lee JM, Morrison DP. Presuicideattempt communications between parasuicides and consulted caregivers. Suicide andLife-Threatening Behavior.1992,22(3):289-302.
    [90] Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McClay J, Mill J,Martin J, Braithwaite A. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by apolymorphism in the5-HTT gene. Science Signalling.2003,301(5631):386.
    [91] Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang K-Y, Eaves L, Hoh J, Griem A, Kovacs M, Ott J,Merikangas KR. Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR),stressful life events, and risk of depression. JAMA: the journal of the AmericanMedical Association.2009,301(23):2462-2471.
    [92]李献云,费立鹏,童永胜,李可进,张亚利,张艳萍,徐东,牛雅娟. Beck自杀意念量表中文版在社区成年人群中应用的信效度.中国心理卫生杂志.2010,24(4):250-255.
    [93] Nock MK, Kessler RC. Prevalence of and risk factors for suicide attempts versussuicide gestures: Analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey. Journal of AbnormalPsychology.2006,115(3):616.
    [94] Joiner Jr TE, Rudd MD. Intensity and duration of suicidal crisis vary as a function ofprevious suicide attempts and negative life events. Journal of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology.2000,68(5):909-916.
    [95] Dean PJ, Range LM. The Escape Theory of Suicide and perfectionism in collegestudents. Death Studies.1996,20(4):415-424.
    [96] Dean PJ, Range LM. Testing the escape theory of suicide in an outpatient clinicalpopulation. Cognitive Therapy and Research.1999,23(6):561-572.
    [97] Rice KG, Ashby JS, Slaney RB. Self-Esteem as a Mediator Between Perfectionismand Depression: A Structural Equations Analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology.1998,45(3):304-314.
    [98] Frost RO, Marten P, Lahart C, Rosenblate R. The dimensions of perfectionism.Cognitive therapy and research.1990,14(5):449-468.
    [99] Shafran R, Mansell W. Perfectionism and psychopathology: a review of research andtreatment. Clinical Psychology Review.2001,21(6):879-906.
    [100] Suddarth BH, Slaney RB. An Investigation of the Dimensions of Perfectionism inCollege Students. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development.2001,34(3):157-165.
    [101] Slaney RB, Rice KG, Mobley M, Trippi J, Ashby JS. The Revised Almost PerfectScale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development.2001,34(3):130-145.
    [102]杨丽,梁宝勇,张秀阁,吴雨晨.近乎完美量表修订版(APS-R)的中文修订.心理与行为研究.2007,5(02):139-144.
    [103] Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journalof health and social behavior.1983:385-396.
    [104] Peterson C, Semmel A, yon Baeyer C, Abramson LY, Metalsky GI, Seligman ME.The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research.1982,6(3):287-300.
    [105]王纯,张宁.归因方式问卷的初步修订.中国行为医学科学.2006,15(05):470-471.
    [106] McDonald PJ, Harris SG, Maher JE. Arousal-induced self-awareness: An artifactualrelationship? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.1983,44(2):285-289.
    [107] Radloff LS. The CES-D scale A self-report depression scale for research in thegeneral population. Applied psychological measurement.1977,1(3):385-401.
    [108]章婕,吴振云,方格,李娟,韩布新,陈祉妍.流调中心抑郁量表全国城市常模的建立.中国心理卫生杂志.2010,24(02):139-143.
    [109] Meyer TJ, Miller ML, Metzger RL, Borkovec TD. Development and validation ofthe Penn State worry questionnaire. Behaviour research and therapy.1990,28(6):487-495.
    [110] Brown TA, Antony MM, Barlow DH. Psychometric properties of the Penn StateWorry Questionnaire in a clinical anxiety disorders sample. Behaviour research andtherapy.1992,30(1):33-37.
    [111]沙丽曼,王纯,刘军,钟杰.宾州忧虑问卷在中国大学生人群中的初步修订.中国临床康复.2006,10(30):24-27.
    [112] Steed LG. A psychometric comparison of four measures of hope and optimism.Educational and Psychological Measurement.2002,62(3):466-482.
    [113] Scheier M, Carver C, Bridges M. Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (andtrait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the Life OrientationTest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.1994,67(6):1063.
    [114]温娟娟.生活定向测验在大学生中的信效度.中国心理卫生杂志.2012,26(04):305-309.
    [115] Gutierrez PM, Osman A, Barrios FX, Kopper BA, Baker MT, Haraburda CM.Development of the reasons for living inventory for young adults. Journal of clinicalpsychology.2002,58(4):339-357.
    [116] Rushton JL, Michelle Forcier M, Schectman RM. Epidemiology of DepressiveSymptoms in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Journal ofAmerican Academy of Child&Adolescent Psychiatry.2002,41(2):199-205.
    [117] Arthur N, Hayward L. The Relationships between Perfectionism, Standards ofAcademic Achievement, and Emotional Distress in Postsecondary Students. Journalof College Student Development.1997,38(6):622-632.
    [118] Hu Lt, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: AMultidisciplinary Journal.1999,6(1):1-55.
    [119] Bentler PM, Chou C-P. Practical issues in structural modeling. SociologicalMethods&Research.1987,16(1):78-117.
    [120] Hewitt PL, Flett GL. Perfectionism in the self and social contexts:Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology.1991,60(3):456-470.
    [121] Konick LC, Gutierrez PM. Testing a model of suicide ideation in college students.Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior.2005,35(2):181-192.
    [122] Kisch J, Leino EV, Silverman MM. Aspects of suicidal behavior, depression, andtreatment in college students: Results from the spring2000national college healthassessment survey. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior.2005,35(1):3-13.
    [123] Blackburn S, Johnston L, Blampied N, Popp D, Kallen R. An application of escapetheory to binge eating. European Eating Disorders Review.2006,14(1):23-31.
    [124] Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, Lee-Chai A, Barndollar K, Tr tschel R. The automatedwill: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology.2001,81(6):1014-1027.
    [125] Greenwald A, Banaji M. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, andstereotypes. Psychological Review.1995,102(1):4.
    [126] Van Orden KA, Witte TK, Cukrowicz KC, Braithwaite SR, Selby EA, Joiner TE.The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological Review.2010,117(2):575-600.
    [127] Meyer DE, Schvaneveldt RW. Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidenceof a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology.1971,90(2):227-234.
    [128] Fazio RH, Sanbonmatsu DM, Powell MC, Kardes FR. On the automatic activationof attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.1986,50(2):229-238.
    [129] Collins AM, Loftus EF. A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing.Psychological Review.1975,82(6):407-428.
    [130] Batson CD, Polycarpou MP, Harmon-Jones E, Imhoff HJ, Mitchener EC, BednarLL, Klein TR, Highberger L. Empathy and attitudes: Can feeling for a member of astigmatized group improve feelings toward the group? Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology.1997,72(1):105-118.
    [131] Rotter JB. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control ofreinforcement. Psychological Monographs.1966,80(1):1-28.
    [132] Tong J, Wang L. Validation of locus of control scale in Chinese organizations.Personality and Individual Differences.2006,41(5):941-950.
    [133] Mazza JJ, Reynolds WM. A longitudinal investigation of depression, hopelessness,social support, and major and minor life events and their relation to suicidal ideationin adolescents. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior.1998,28(4):358-374.
    [134] Tyssen R, Vaglum P, Gr nvold NT, Ekeberg. Suicidal ideation among medicalstudents and young physicians: a nationwide and prospective study of prevalenceand predictors. Journal of Affective Disorders.2001,64(1):69-79.
    [135] Shaw J, Turnbull P. Suicide in custody. Psychiatry.2009,8(7):265-268.
    [136] Cheng IC, Hu FC, Tseng MC. Inpatient suicide in a general hospital. GeneralHospital Psychiatry.2009,31(2):110-115.
    [137] Schneider B, Grebner K, Schnabel A, Hampel H, Georgi K, Seidler A. Impact ofemployment status and work-related factors on risk of completed suicide. Acase-control psychological autopsy study. Psychiatry Research.2011,190(2-3):265-270.
    [138] Dijksterhuis A, Bargh JA. The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effectsof social perception on social behavior. Advances in Experimental SocialPsychology.2001,33:1-40.
    [139] Loersch C, Payne BK. The situated inference model: An integrative account of theeffects of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation. Perspectives onPsychological Science.2011,6(3):234-252.
    [140] Wheeler SC, Demarree KG, Petty RE. Understanding the role of the self inprime-to-behavior effects: the Active-Self account. Personality and SocialPsychology Review.2007,11(3):234-261.
    [141] Hurtado E, Haye A, González R, Manes F, Ibá ez A. Contextual blending ofingroup/outgroup face stimuli and word valence: LPP modulation and convergenceof measures. BMC neuroscience.2009,10(1):69.
    [142] Williams JK, Themanson JR. Neural correlates of the implicit association test:evidence for semantic and emotional processing. Social Cognitive and AffectiveNeuroscience.2011,6(4):468-476.
    [143]徐莹,张庆林.不同程度吸烟者内隐态度测量范式下的ERP研究.心理科学.2011,34(3):709-713.
    [144] Klauer KC, Mierke J. Task-set inertia, attitude accessibility, and compatibility-ordereffects: New evidence for a task-set switching account of the Implicit AssociationTest effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.2005,31(2):208-217.
    [145] Goffaux P, Phillips N, Sinai M, Pushkar D. Behavioural and electrophysiologicalmeasures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions. Biologicalpsychology.2006,72(3):278-290.
    [146] Mierke J, Klauer KC. Implicit association measurement with the IAT: Evidence foreffects of executive control processes. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie.2001,48(2):107-122.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700